Jump to content

Effects of alcohol on memory

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
(Redirected from Effect of Alcohol on Memory)

Ethanol-structure.svg
Ethanol
Alcoholic_beverages.jpg
Alcoholic beverages

Effects of alcohol on memory include disruption of various memory processes, affecting both formation and recall of information.

Mode of actions

[edit]

Effects on the hippocampus

[edit]

Alcohol acts as a general central nervous system depressant, but it also affects some specific areas of the brain to a greater extent than others. Memory impairment caused by alcohol has been linked to the disruption of hippocampal function—particularly affecting gamma-Aminobutyric acid (GABA) and N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) neurotransmission which negatively impacts long-term potentiation (LTP).[1] The molecular basis of LTP is associated with learning and memory.[2] Particularly, damage to hippocampal CA1 cells adversely affects memory formation,[3] and this disruption has been linked to dose-dependent levels of alcohol consumption.[4] At higher doses, alcohol significantly inhibits neuronal activity in both the CA1 and CA3 pyramidal cell layers of the hippocampus.[5][6][7] This impairs memory encoding,[8] since the hippocampus plays an important role in the formations of new memories.

A hippocampal pyramidal cell

Molecular effects on GABA and NMDA receptors

[edit]
A GABAA receptor

Alcohol also acts as a positive allosteric modulator of GABA receptors, specifically type GABAA.[9] Upon activation, these GABA receptors conduct Cl-, resulting in neuronal hyperpolarization. This hyperpolarization decreases the chance of an action potential occurring and thus, it has an inhibitory effect on neurotransmission in the central nervous system. GABAA receptor subtypes vary in their sensitivities to dosage of alcohol consumed. Furthermore, acute alcohol intake promotes GABAergic neurotransmission via the presynaptic release of GABA, the dephosphorylation of GABAA receptors (increasing GABA sensitivity), and the elevation of endogenous GABAergic neuroactive steroids.[10] Protein kinase C (PKC) has been implicated in differentially modulating the response of the GABAA receptor to alcohol, with effects depending on the PKC isozyme.[11] Alcohol effects have also implicated protein kinase A in affecting GABAA receptor function, such as promoting sensitivity.[12] Enhancement of GABAergic transmission due to alcohol consumption can also be brought about by neuroactive steroids, such as allopregnanolone, which act as GABAA receptor agonists.[10][13] Both chronic alcohol consumption and alcohol dependence are correlated with the altered expression, properties, and functions of the GABAA receptor that may contribute to alcohol tolerance.[10] There is still much yet to be discovered about alcohol's specific and varying effects on both the GABAA receptor and its subtypes.

At higher doses, ethanol also affects NMDA receptors (NMDARs) by inhibiting the ion current induced by NMDA, a glutamate receptor agonist.[14] This inhibition of synaptic excitation by alcohol has been shown to be dose-dependent (up to a certain point, after which it did not differ by much).[15] Alcohol appears to produce this inhibition by using a site of the NMDAR that is accessible from the extracellular environment.[16] Therefore, this inhibition of an ion current usually produced by NMDAR activation leads to decreased LTP in hippocampal areas.[17] Alcohol negatively affects LTP to a greater degree in immature versus mature animals.[18] In adolescents, alcohol decreases the expression of both the NMDAR NR2A subunit in the hippocampus and the NR1 subunit in the prefrontal cortex.[19] Studies have also found that a decrease in phosphorylation of 2B subunit in the prefrontal cortex, the hippocampus, the nucleus accumbens, and the striatum.[20] NMDARS may be affected by PKA regulation due to the actions of alcohol.[21] Alcohol's effects on GABAA neurotransmission may indirectly inhibit the activity of the NMDAR, and they may contribute to its blockade of LTP induction; however, alcohol's direct effects on NMDAR alone are sufficient for the inhibition of LTP.[22] The varying dose-dependent response to alcohol relies on the combined interactions and responses of the GABAA receptors, NMDARs, and metabotropic glutamate receptors subtype 5 (mGluR5).[23][24][25] These changes prevent excitatory synaptic transmissions from occurring, affecting synaptic plasticity and, in turn, memory and learning. However, there is still much yet to be elucidated concerning specific molecular mechanisms of how alcohol affects memory formation.

Effects on other brain regions

[edit]

Alcohol also impairs and alters the functioning in the cerebellum, which affects both motor function and coordination.[26] It has a notable inhibitory effect on the neurons of the cerebral cortex, affecting and altering thought processes, decreasing inhibition, and increasing the pain threshold. It also decreases sexual performance by depressing nerve centers in the hypothalamus.[27][28] Alcohol also has an effect on urine excretion via inhibition of anti-diuretic hormone (ADH) secretion of the pituitary gland. Lastly, it depresses breathing and heart rate by inhibiting neuronal functioning of the medulla.[29]

Long-term memory

[edit]

Long-term memory (LTM) has both a long duration and a large capacity.[30] Memories that are stored in LTM can last from a few days to a lifetime.[30] LTM consists of both explicit memory (requiring conscious awareness) and implicit memory (unconscious awareness).[30] Information selected for LTM goes through three processes. First of all, in the encoding stage, information from the senses is incorporated into mental activity in the form of a memory.[30] Secondly, storage involves taking this information and holding it indefinitely in memory.[30] Lastly, retrieval is the ability to recall information from the long-term memory storage. Each of these processes can be affected by alcohol.[30]

Hippocampus.gif
Animation: Hippocampus (red)

Explicit memory

[edit]

Explicit memory requires conscious and intentional effort for recall.[30] It includes both episodic memory (for specific events, such as a party) and semantic memory (for general information, such as one's name).[30]

Alcohol impairs episodic encoding, specifically for cued recall, recognition of completed word fragments, and free recall.[31] A blackout is an example of a difficulty in encoding episodic memories due to alcohol. Blackouts are caused by a rapid increase in blood alcohol concentration (BAC) which in turn distorts the neurons in the hippocampus.[32] This distortion impairs a person's ability to form new episodic memories.[32]

High doses of alcohol severely disrupt the storage process of semantic memories.[33] Alcohol was found to impair the storage of novel stimuli but not that of previously learned information.[33] Since alcohol affects the central nervous system, it hinders semantic storage functioning by restricting the consolidation of the information from encoding.[33]

Retrieval of explicit memory is significantly impaired by alcohol. When compared to sober participants, intoxicated participants performed quite poorly on a recall task for everyday events (i.e., episodic memory).[34] Intoxicated participants are also slower to respond in reaction time tasks.[35] Alcohol also impairs retrieval in word recognition tasks.[31] When both encoding and retrieval take place during intoxication, there are surprisingly more impairments for cued recall than for free recall.[31] In terms of gender differences in retrieval processes, females tend to score lower than males on recall tasks when intoxicated.[35]

Implicit memory

[edit]

Implicit memory does not require conscious effort or intention for recall.[30] It occurs when previous experience influences performance on a certain task.[30] This is evident in priming experiments. Implicit memory includes procedural memory, which influences our everyday behaviours, such as riding a bike or tying shoes.[30] People can perform these abilities without even thinking about them, which means procedural memory functions automatically. While retrieval of explicit memory is severely impaired by alcohol, retrieval of implicit memory is not.[34] Intoxicated subjects score higher on recognition tasks (involving implicit memory) than they can on recall tasks (involving explicit memory).[34]

Short-term memory

[edit]

Short-term memory refers to the temporary storage of small amounts of information over short delays.[30] Digit span refers to the proposed number of pieces of information (5-9) that can be held in short-term memory. This is also referred to as the magic number seven—plus or minus two. Any more pieces of information than this, and newer items replace previous items.[36] Alcohol intoxication has been found to have dissociative effects on both short-term memory and cognitive functioning.[37][38]

Gray727_anterior_cingulate_cortex.png
Anterior cingulate cortex (yellow)
Parietal_lobe_animation_small.gif
Animation: Parietal lobe (red)

Brain areas affected by alcohol

[edit]

Alcohol affects the functioning of the brain. Neurochemical changes occurring in the anterior cingulate are correlated with altered short-term memory functions in the brains of young alcoholic men.[39] fMRIs of alcohol-dependent women displayed significantly less blood oxygen in the frontal and parietal regions, especially in the right hemisphere.[40] This is supported by findings of short-term memory impairment by lesions of both the parietal lobe[41] and the prefrontal cortex.[42] Associations between third ventricle volume and cognitive performance on memory tests have been found in alcoholics.[43] Specifically, increases in third ventricular volume correlate with a decline in memory performance.[43]

Tasks and intoxication findings

[edit]

Short-term memory is commonly tested with visual tasks. Short-term memory, especially for non-verbal and spatial material, are impaired by intoxication.[43] Alcohol decreases iconic memory (a type of visual short-term memory).[44] With BACs between 80 and 84 mg/dl, more intrusion errors occur in a delayed recall task compared to a control group.[45] Intrusion errors, which represent reflective cognitive functioning, occur when irrelevant information is produced. Alcoholics have less control of inhibiting intrusions.[45] Acute alcohol intoxication in social drinkers caused more intrusion errors in delayed recall tasks than in immediate free recall tasks.[45] Acute alcohol intoxication increases the susceptibility to interference, which allows for more intrusion errors when there is a short delay.[45] Free recall (given list of words then asked to recall list) is significantly lower and therefore impaired by alcohol intoxication.[46][47] Encoding deficits were found in verbal free recall and recognition tasks under the influence of alcohol.[48] A discrimination task found significant alcohol-related impairments both in depth perception and in visual short-term memory.[49] State-dependent learning and relearning studies in male heavy drinkers demonstrate that the condition of intoxication while learning and sobriety when tested caused a performance deficit in free recall tasks.[50] These findings are supportive of alcohol-induced storage deficits (not retrieval deficits).[50] The effects of acute alcohol consumption on visual short-term memory, stereoscopic depth perception, and attention were all studied. A 33% alcohol condition showed significant impairments both in depth perception and in visual short-term memory (assessed by the vernier discrimination task).[51]

Working_memory_model.svg
Working Memory Model. Alcohol intoxication can disrupt rehearsal strategies which may involve the phonological loop and/or the visuospatial sketchpad.

Effects on working memory

[edit]

Working memory allows one to keep things in mind while simultaneously performing complex tasks.[30] It involves a system for both the temporary storage and the manipulation of information, subsequently forming a crucial link between perception and controlled action.[52] Evidence suggests that working memory involves three components: the central executive which controls attention, the visuo-spatial sketchpad which holds and manipulates spatial information, and the phonological loop which performs a similar function for auditory and speech-based information.[52]

In the short term

[edit]

Alcohol consumption has substantial, measurable effects on working memory, although these effects vary greatly between individual responses. Not much is really known about the neural mechanisms that underlie these individual differences.[53] It is also found that alcohol impairs working memory by affecting mnemonic strategies and executive processes rather than by shrinking the basic holding capacity of working memory. Isolated acute-moderate levels of alcohol intoxication do not physically alter the structures that are critical for working memory function, such as the frontal cortex, the parietal cortex, the anterior cingulate, and parts of the basal ganglia.[37] One finding regarding the effects of alcohol on working memory points out that alcohol reduces working memory only in individuals with a high baseline working memory capacity,[54] which suggests that alcohol might not uniformly affect working memory in many different individuals. Alcohol appears to impair the capacity of working memory to modulate response inhibition.[54] Alcohol disinhibits behaviour, but it only does so in individuals with a low baseline working memory capacity.[54] An interesting finding is that the incentive to perform well with working memory measurement tasks while under the influence of alcohol 'does, in fact, have some effect on working memory, as it boosts scores in the rate of mental scanning and reaction time to stimulus; however, it did not reduce the number of errors as opposed to subjects with no incentive to perform well.[55] Even acute alcohol intoxication (a blood alcohol concentration of 0.08-0.09%) produces a substantial impairment of working memory processes that require mnemonic rehearsal strategies.[37] It is less likely for alcohol to impair a working memory task that does not rely on memory rehearsal or associated mnemonic strategies.[37] Because of this, working memory is very susceptible to falter when an individual participates in tasks involving retention concerning both auditory and visual sequences.[37]

An interesting example of this is the failure of guitarists or other musicians performing concerts to cue in on auditory patterns and make it known that their performance is hindered by intoxication, whereas professional basketball (a less sequence-heavy activity for working memory) standout Ron Artest admitted in an interview with Sporting News to drinking heavily during half-time early in his career and the fact that it had little—if not any recognizable—effect on his working memory. His former coach Fran Fraschilla has gone on record saying:[56]

It's a surprise because every day at practice, he came out in a mood to play. He came out in a basketball rage. He was fully committed; he wanted to let our upperclassmen know that he was the alpha male. It never came up that he had any sort of a problem with alcohol. This is the first I've heard of it.

In the long term

[edit]

Alcohol has been shown to have just some long-term effects on working memory. Findings have shown that in order for working memory to be substantially affected, long-term heavy drinking must be sustained over a long period of time, as up to one drink per day does not impair any cognitive function and may actually decrease the risk of a cognitive decline.[57] Furthermore, chronic alcoholism is associated with the impairment in both sustained attention and visual working memory. As a result, alcoholics have reduced ability, but not necessarily inability, to perform these executive tasks. This is assumed to be subserved by regions of the prefrontal cortex.[58] While it may not serve as a surprise that chronic alcoholism is linked to any decreased cognitive function such as working memory, one surprising finding is not only that even moderate levels of alcohol consumption during pregnancy were shown to have an adverse effect on the child's working memory when tested at 7.5 years of age, but also that working memory may be the most important aspect of attention that is adversely affected by prenatal alcohol exposure.[59]

Prospective memory

[edit]

Prospective memory involves remembering to carry out an intended action in the future without an explicit reminder.[30] Alcohol has been found to impair this ability. Chronic heavy alcohol users report significantly more prospective forgetting compared to low-dose and alcohol-free controls.[60] The Prospective Memory Questionnaire assesses short-term habitual prospective memory, long-term episodic prospective memory, and internally cued prospective memory.[60] Chronic heavy alcohol users reported significantly greater deficits for all three aspects of prospective memory.[60] Individuals that report heavy alcohol use report 24% more difficulties with prospective memory than those who report that they are light drinkers and 30% more difficulties than those who report that they never drink.[61] The effects of alcohol on prospective memory can also be assessed in the laboratory by simulating prospective memory tasks that individuals face in everyday life. Individuals who are given 0.6 g/kg alcohol prior to performing prospective memory tasks do significantly poorer than a placebo group.[62] Alcohol can damage the prefrontal and frontal areas of the brain, and this may be responsible for prospective memory impairments since prospective memory performance is highly correlated with frontal executive functions.[60]

[edit]

The memory inhibiting effects of alcohol are often a prominent topic in popular culture. It appears in movies, books, and television shows. Several movies show characters drinking alcohol to the point of memory loss and awakening the next morning with a host of problems due to actions they performed while intoxicated.

One example is The Hangover, where three groomsmen lose the groom during a bachelor party in Las Vegas, so they retrace their steps to find him.[63] The characters still had functioning implicit/procedural memory, which allowed them to carry out the many acts they performed that night, but their episodic memory was impaired and thus they had no recollection of the events occurring. In addition to alcohol the characters were also under the influence of flunitrazepam.

Another movie is What Happens in Vegas. After an intoxicated night in "Sin City," two people wake-up to find they got married.[64]

Songs such as Waking Up in Vegas by Katy Perry[65] and Last Name by Carrie Underwood[66] also depict characters waking up and not remembering the night before due to alcohol consumption.

By some accounts, popular culture makes light of the memory problems that can result from alcohol consumption.

The court case R. v. Daviault [1994] concerned the viability of a legal defense based on intoxication.

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Rose, M. E.; Grant, J. E. (2010). "Alcohol-Induced Blackout". Journal of Addiction Medicine. 4 (2): 61–73. doi:10.1097/ADM.0b013e3181e1299d. PMID 21769024. S2CID 23068837.
  2. ^ Bliss, T. V. P.; Collingridge, G. L. (1993). "A synaptic model of memory: Long-term potentiation in the hippocampus". Nature. 361 (6407): 31–39. Bibcode:1993Natur.361...31B. doi:10.1038/361031a0. PMID 8421494. S2CID 4326182.
  3. ^ Zola-Morgan, S.; Squire, L. R.; Amaral, D. G. (1986). "Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: Enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippocampus". The Journal of Neuroscience. 6 (10): 2950–2967. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.06-10-02950.1986. PMC 6568782. PMID 3760943.
  4. ^ White, A. M.; Best, P. J. (2000). "Effects of ethanol on hippocampal place-cell and interneuron activity". Brain Research. 876 (1–2): 154–165. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02629-9. PMID 10973604. S2CID 37879454.
  5. ^ Ryabinin AE, Criado JR, Henriksen SJ, Bloom FE, Wilson MC (1997). Differential sensitivity of c-Fos expression in hippocampus and other brain regions to moderate and low doses of alcohol. Mol Psychiatry, (1),32-43.
  6. ^ Ryabinin, AE (1998). "Role of hippocampus in alcohol-induced memory impairment: implications from behavioral and immediate early gene studies". Psychopharmacology. 139 (1–2): 1–2. doi:10.1007/s002130050687. PMID 9768540. S2CID 22730822.
  7. ^ Weiner, JL; Gu, C; Dunwiddie, TV (1997). "Differential Ethanol Sensitivity of Subpopulations of GABAA Synapses Onto Rat Hippocampal CA1 Pyramidal Neurons". The Journal of Neurophysiology. 77 (3): 1306–1312. doi:10.1152/jn.1997.77.3.1306. PMID 9084598.
  8. ^ White, A (2003). "What Happened? Alcohol, Memory Blackouts and the Brain". Alcohol Research & Health. 27 (2): 186–196.
  9. ^ Paul, S. M. (2006). "Alcohol-sensitive GABA receptors and alcohol antagonists". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 103 (22): 8307–8308. Bibcode:2006PNAS..103.8307P. doi:10.1073/pnas.0602862103. PMC 1482489. PMID 16717187..
  10. ^ a b c Kumar, S.; Porcu, P.; Werner, D. F.; Matthews, D. B.; Diaz-Granados, J. L.; Helfand, R. S.; Morrow, A. L. (2009). "The role of GABAA receptors in the acute and chronic effects of ethanol: A decade of progress". Psychopharmacology. 205 (4): 529–564. doi:10.1007/s00213-009-1562-z. PMC 2814770. PMID 19455309.
  11. ^ Proctor, W. R.; Poelchen, W.; Bowers, B. J.; Wehner, J. M.; Messing, R. O.; Dunwiddie, T. V. (2003). "Ethanol Differentially Enhances Hippocampal GABAA Receptor-Mediated Responses in Protein Kinase Cgamma (PKCgamma ) and PKCepsilon Null Mice". Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 305 (1): 264–270. doi:10.1124/jpet.102.045450. PMID 12649378. S2CID 6395717.
  12. ^ Park, S. K.; Sedore, S. A.; Cronmiller, C.; Hirsh, J. (2000). "Type II cAMP-dependent Protein Kinase-deficient Drosophila Are Viable but Show Developmental, Circadian, and Drug Response Phenotypes". Journal of Biological Chemistry. 275 (27): 20588–20596. doi:10.1074/jbc.M002460200. PMID 10781603.
  13. ^ Silvers, J. M.; Tokunaga, S.; Berry, R. B.; White, A. M.; Matthews, D. B. (2003). "Impairments in spatial learning and memory: Ethanol, allopregnanolone, and the hippocampus". Brain Research. Brain Research Reviews. 43 (3): 275–284. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2003.09.002. PMID 14629930. S2CID 43784032.
  14. ^ Lovinger, DM; White, G; Weight, FF (1989). "Ethanol inhibits NMDA-activated ion current in hippocampal neurons". Science. 243 (4899): 1721–1724. Bibcode:1989Sci...243.1721L. doi:10.1126/science.2467382. PMID 2467382.
  15. ^ Lovinger, D. M.; White, G.; Weight, F. F. (1990). "NMDA receptor-mediated synaptic excitation selectively inhibited by ethanol in hippocampal slice from adult rat". The Journal of Neuroscience. 10 (4): 1372–1379. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.10-04-01372.1990. PMC 6570208. PMID 2158533.
  16. ^ Peoples, R. W.; Stewart, R. R. (2000). "Alcohols inhibit N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors via a site exposed to the extracellular environment". Neuropharmacology (Submitted manuscript). 39 (10): 1681–1691. doi:10.1016/S0028-3908(00)00067-8. PMID 10884550. S2CID 26168622.
  17. ^ Morrisett, R. A.; Swartzwelder, H. S. (1993). "Attenuation of hippocampal long-term potentiation by ethanol: A patch-clamp analysis of glutamatergic and GABAergic mechanisms". The Journal of Neuroscience. 13 (5): 2264–2272. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.13-05-02264.1993. PMC 6576561. PMID 8478698.
  18. ^ Swartzwelder, H. S.; Wilson, W. A.; Tayyeb, M. I. (1995). "Age-dependent inhibition of long-term potentiation by ethanol in immature versus mature hippocampus". Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 19 (6): 1480–1485. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb01011.x. PMID 8749814.
  19. ^ Pian, J. P.; Criado, J. R.; Milner, R.; Ehlers, C. L. (2010). "NMDAR subunit expression in adult and adolescent brain following chronic ethanol exposure". Neuroscience. 170 (2): 645–654. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.06.065. PMC 2945397. PMID 20603193.
  20. ^ Pascual, M.; Boix, J.; Felipo, V.; Guerri, C. (2009). "Repeated alcohol administration during adolescence causes changes in the mesolimbic dopaminergic and glutamatergic systems and promotes alcohol intake in the adult rat". Journal of Neurochemistry. 108 (4): 920–931. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05835.x. PMID 19077056.
  21. ^ Lai, C. C.; Kuo, T. I.; Lin, H. H. (2007). "The Role of Protein Kinase a in Acute Ethanol-Induced Neurobehavioral Actions in Rats". Anesthesia & Analgesia. 105 (1): 89–96. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.333.5710. doi:10.1213/01.ane.0000263030.13249.36. PMID 17578962. S2CID 15529888.
  22. ^ Schummers, J.; Browning, M. D. (2001). "Evidence for a role for GABA(A) and NMDA receptors in ethanol inhibition of long-term potentiation". Brain Research. Molecular Brain Research. 94 (1–2): 9–14. doi:10.1016/s0169-328x(01)00161-9. PMID 11597760.
  23. ^ Besheer, J.; Cox, A. A.; Hodge, C. W. (2003). "Coregulation of Ethanol Discrimination by the Nucleus Accumbens and Amygdala". Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 27 (3): 450–456. doi:10.1097/01.ALC.0000057036.64169.C1. PMID 12658110.
  24. ^ Besheer, J.; Hodge, C. W. (2004). "Pharmacological and Anatomical Evidence for an Interaction Between mGluR5- and GABAA α1-Containing Receptors in the Discriminative Stimulus Effects of Ethanol". Neuropsychopharmacology. 30 (4): 747–757. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300616. PMC 2892057. PMID 15549054.
  25. ^ Hodge, C. W.; Cox, A. A. (1998). "The discriminative stimulus effects of ethanol are mediated by NMDA and GABA(A) receptors in specific limbic brain regions". Psychopharmacology. 139 (1–2): 95–107. doi:10.1007/s002130050694. PMID 9768547. S2CID 38391993.
  26. ^ Ming, Z; Criswell, HE; Yu, G; Breese, GR (2006). "Competing presynaptic and postsynaptic effects of ethanol on cerebellar purkinje neurons". Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 30 (8): 1400–1407. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00167.x. PMC 2949273. PMID 16899043.
  27. ^ Waltman, C; Blevins Jr, LS; Boyd, G; Wand, GS (1993). "The effects of mild ethanol intoxication on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in nonalcoholic men". Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 77 (2): 518–522. doi:10.1210/jcem.77.2.8393888. PMID 8393888.
  28. ^ Halpern-Felsher, BL; Millstein, SG; Ellen, JM (1996). "Relationship of alcohol use and risky sexual behavior: a review and analysis of findings". Journal of Adolescent Health. 19 (5): 331–336. doi:10.1016/s1054-139x(96)00024-9. PMID 8934293.
  29. ^ Sun, MK; Reis, DJ (1992). "Effects of systemic ethanol on medullary vasomotor neurons and baroreflexes". Neuroscience Letters. 137 (2): 232–236. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(92)90411-y. PMID 1584465. S2CID 45576960.
  30. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n Baddeley, A., Eysenck, M.W. and Anderson, M.C. (2009). Memory. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  31. ^ a b c Soderlund, H.; Parker, E.S.; Schwartz, B.L.; Tulving, E. (2005). "Memory encoding and retrieval on the ascending and descending limbs of the blood alcohol concentration curve". Psychopharmacology. 182 (2): 305–317. doi:10.1007/s00213-005-0096-2. PMID 16160875. S2CID 2084273.
  32. ^ a b Lee, H.; Roh, S.; Kim, D. (2009). "Alcohol-Induced Blackout". International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 6 (11): 2783–2792. doi:10.3390/ijerph6112783. PMC 2800062. PMID 20049223.
  33. ^ a b c Parker, Birnbaum; Noble (1976). "Alcohol and Memory: Storage and State Dependency". Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 15 (6): 691–702. doi:10.1016/0022-5371(76)90061-x.
  34. ^ a b c Nelson, T.; McSpadden, M.; Fromme, K.; Marlatt, G. (1986). "Effects of Alcohol Intoxication on Metamemory and on Retrieval from Long-Term Memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 115 (3): 247–254. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.115.3.247. PMID 2944987.
  35. ^ a b Haut, J.; Beckwith, B.; Petros, T.; Russell, S. (1989). "Gender Differences in Retrieval From Long-Term Memory Following Acute Intoxication With Ethanol". Physiology & Behavior. 45 (6): 1161–1165. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(89)90103-0. PMID 2813540. S2CID 40873039.
  36. ^ Miller, G.A. (1994). "The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information". Psychological Review. 101 (2): 343–352. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.101.2.343. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002C-4646-B. PMID 8022966.
  37. ^ a b c d e Saults, J.S.; Cowan, N.; Sher, K.J.; Moreno, M.V. (2007). "Differential short Term effects of Alcohol on Working Memory: Distinguishing Multiple Processes". Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 15 (6): 576–587. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.15.6.576. PMC 2658822. PMID 18179311.
  38. ^ Bates, M. E. (2006). "Acute alcohol effects on repetition priming and word recognition memory with equivalent memory cues". Brain Cogn. 60 (2): 118–127. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2005.07.009. PMID 16377048. S2CID 41154421.
  39. ^ Eun, L.; Dong-Pyo, J.; Jae-Jin, K.; Suk Kyoon, A.; Sangjin, P.; In-Young, K.; Sun, I.K.; Kang-Jun, Y.; Kee, N. (2007). "Alteration of brain metabolites in young alcoholics without structural changes". NeuroReport. 18 (14): 1511–1514. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282ef7625. PMID 17712285. S2CID 20409183.
  40. ^ Tapert, S.F.; Brown, G.G.; Kindermann, S.S.; Cheung, E.H.; Frank, L.R.; Brown, S.A. (2006). "fMRI measurement of brain dysfunctions in alcohol-dependent young women". Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 25 (2): 26–245.
  41. ^ Warrington, E. K.; James, M.; Maciejewski, C. (1986). "The WAIS as a lateralizing and localizing diagnostic instrument: A study of 656 patients with unilateral cerebral lesions". Neuropsychologia. 24 (2): 223–239. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(86)90055-2. PMID 3714027. S2CID 33190123.
  42. ^ Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1992). "Working memory and the mind". Scientific American. 267 (3): 110–117. Bibcode:1992SciAm.267c.110G. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0992-110. PMID 1502513.
  43. ^ a b c Sullivan, E.V.; Rosenbloom, M.J.; Lim, K.O.; Pfefferbam, A. (2000). "Longitudinal changes in cognition, gait, and balance in abstinent and relapsed alcoholic men: relationships to changes in brain structure". Neuropsychology. 14 (2): 178–188. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.14.2.178. PMID 10791858.
  44. ^ Subhan (1983). "The effects of midazolam in conjunction with alcohol in iconic memory and free recall". Neuropsychobiology. 9 (4): 230–234. doi:10.1159/000117970. PMID 6646395.
  45. ^ a b c d Ray, S.; Bates, M.E. (2007). "Acute effects of alcohol on intrusion errors in free recall tasks". International Journal on Disability and Human Development. 6 (2): 201–205. doi:10.1515/ijdhd.2007.6.2.201. S2CID 147314783.
  46. ^ Hashtroudi, S.; Parker, DeLisi; Wyatt, R.J.; Mutter, S.A. (1984). "Intact retention in acute alcohol amnesia". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 10 (1): 156–163. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.10.1.156. PMID 6242732.
  47. ^ Tracy, J.I.; Bates, M.E. (1999). "The selective effects of alcohol on automatic and effortful memory processes". Neuropsychology. 13 (2): 282–290. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.13.2.282. PMID 10353377.
  48. ^ Williams, H.L.; Rundell, U.H. (1984). "Effect of alcohol on recall and recognition as functions of processing levels". Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 45 (1): 10–15. doi:10.15288/jsa.1984.45.10. PMID 6700218.
  49. ^ Wegner, A.J.; Fahle, M. (1999). "Alcohol and visual performance". Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 23 (3): 465–482. doi:10.1016/S0278-5846(99)00009-3. PMID 10378230. S2CID 24469283.
  50. ^ a b Miller, M.E.; Adesso, V.J.; Fleming, J.P.; Gino, A.; Lauerman, R. (1978). "Effects of alcohol on the storage and retrieval processes of heavy social drinkers". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory. 4 (3): 246–255. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.4.3.246. PMID 660094.
  51. ^ Wegner, A.J.; Fahle, M. (1999). "Alcohol and visual performance". Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 23 (3): 465–482. doi:10.1016/S0278-5846(99)00009-3. PMID 10378230. S2CID 24469283.
  52. ^ a b Baddeley, A. (1998) Comptes Rendus de l'Académie des Sciences – Série III, 321(2-3), 167-173.
  53. ^ Paulus, Martin P, Tapert, Susan F, Pulido, Carmen, & Schuckit, Marc A. (2006). Alcohol attenuates load-related activation during a working memory task: Relation to level of response to alcohol. Dept of Psychiatry, University of California. PP 2
  54. ^ a b c Finn, P.R.; Justus, A.; Mazas, C.; Steinmetz, J.E. (1999). "Working memory, executive processes and the effects of alcohol on Go/No-Go learning: testing a model of behavioral regulation and impulsivity". Psychopharmacology. 146 (4): 465–472. doi:10.1007/pl00005492. PMID 10550497. S2CID 23894568.
  55. ^ Grattan-Miscio, K.E. and Vogel-Sprott, M. (2005) Effects of alcohol and performance incentives on immediate working memory. Springer Berlin / Heidelberg, 188-196
  56. ^ Post Staff Report (3 December 2009). "Artest took drinking from St. John's to NBA halftimes". New York Post. Archived from the original on 18 December 2021. Retrieved 18 December 2021.
  57. ^ Stampfer MJ, Kang JH, Chen J, Cherry R, Grodstein F (2005). "Effects of moderate alcohol consumption on cognitive function in women". N Engl J Med. 352 (3): 245–253. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa041152. PMID 15659724.
  58. ^ Pfefferbaum A, Desmond JE, Galloway C, Menon V, Glover GH, Sullivan EV (July 2001). "Reorganization of frontal systems used by alcoholics for spatial working memory: an fMRI study". NeuroImage. 14 (1 pt 1): 7–20. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0785. PMID 11525339. S2CID 508770.
  59. ^ Burden MJ, Jacobson SW, Sokol RJ, Jacobson JL (March 2005). "Effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on attention and working memory at 7.5 years of age". Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 29 (3): 443–52. doi:10.1097/01.ALC.0000156125.50577.EC. PMID 15770121.
  60. ^ a b c d Heffernan, T.M.; Moss, M; Ling, J. (2002). "Subjective Ratings of Prospective Memory Deficits in Chronic Heavy Alcohol Users". Alcohol and Alcoholism. 37 (3): 269–271. doi:10.1093/alcalc/37.3.269. PMID 12003916.
  61. ^ Ling, J.; Heffernan, T.M.; Buchanan, T.; Rodgers, J.; Scholey, A.B.; Parrot, A.C. (2003). "Effects of Alcohol on Subjective Ratings of Prospective and Everyday Memory Deficits" (PDF). Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 27 (6): 970–974. doi:10.1097/01.alc.0000071741.63467.cb. PMID 12824818.
  62. ^ Leitz, J.R.; Morgan, C. A. J.; Bisby, J.A.; Rendell, P.G.; Curran, V.H. (2009). "Global impairment of prospective memory following acute alcohol". Psychopharmacology. 205 (3): 379–387. doi:10.1007/s00213-009-1546-z. PMID 19440700. S2CID 21923512.
  63. ^ "The Hangover" (2009). Retrieved from https://www.imdb.com/title/tt1119646/.
  64. ^ "What Happens in Vegas" (2008). Retrieved from https://www.imdb.com/title/tt1033643/plotsummary.
  65. ^ "YouTube – Waking Up in Vegas Lyrics – Katy Perry" (2009). Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=06Qf7GdAF70
  66. ^ "YouTube – Carrie Underwood – Last Name" (2009). Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=f27zNlmRMWU