Jump to content

Valence (psychology)

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Valence, also known as hedonic tone, is a characteristic of emotions that determines their emotional affect (intrinsic appeal or repulsion).

Positive valence corresponds to the "goodness" or attractiveness of an object, event, or situation, making it appealing or desirable. Conversely, negative valence relates to "badness" or averseness, rendering something unappealing or undesirable.

This concept is not only used to describe the intrinsic qualities of objects and events but also categorizes emotions based on their inherent attractiveness or averseness.[1][2]

History

[edit]

The use of the term in psychology entered English with the translation from German ("Valenz") in 1935 of works of Kurt Lewin. The original German word suggests "binding", and is commonly used in a grammatical context to describe the ability of one word to semantically and syntactically link another, especially the ability of a verb to require a number of additional terms (e.g. subject and object) to form a complete sentence.

The term chemical valence has been used in physics and chemistry to describe the mechanism by which atoms bind to one another since the nineteenth century.

Phenomenology

[edit]

Valence is an inferred criterion from instinctively generated emotions; it is the property specifying whether feelings/affects are positive, negative or neutral.[2] The existence of at least temporarily unspecified valence is an issue for psychological researchers who reject the existence of neutral emotions (e.g. surprise, sublimation).[2] However, other psychological researchers assume that neutral emotions exist.[3] Two contrasting views in the phenomenology of valence are that of a constrained valence psychology, where the most intense experiences are generally no more than 10 times more intense than the mildest, and the Heavy-Tailed Valence hypothesis, which states that the range of possible degrees of valence is far more extreme.[4]

Measurement

[edit]

Valence could be assigned a number and treated as if it were measured, but the validity of a measurement based on a subjective report is questionable. Measurement based on observations of facial expressions, using the Facial Action Coding System and microexpressions (see Paul Ekman) or muscle activity detected through facial electromyography, or on modern functional brain imaging may overcome this objection. The perceived emotional valence of a facial expression is represented in the right posterior superior temporal sulcus and medial prefrontal cortex.[5]

Examples

[edit]

"Negative" emotions like anger and fear have a negative valence.[6] But positive emotions like joy have a positive valence. Positively valenced emotions are evoked by positively valenced events, objects, or situations.[7] The term is also used to describe the hedonic tone of feelings, certain behaviors (for example, approach and avoidance), goal attainment or non-attainment, and conformity with or violation of norms. Ambivalence can be viewed as conflict between positive and negative valence-carriers.

Theorists taking a valence-based approach to study affect, judgment, and choice posit that emotions with the same valence (e.g., anger and fear or pride and surprise) produce a similar influence on judgments and choices. Suffering is negative valence and the opposite of this is pleasure or happiness.

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Nico H. Frijda, The Emotions. Cambridge(UK): Cambridge University Press, 1986. p. 207
  2. ^ a b c Vazard J (2022). "Feeling the unknown: emotions of uncertainty and their valence". Erkenntnis. 89 (4): 1275–1294. doi:10.1007/s10670-022-00583-1. S2CID 250417356.
  3. ^ Hu, C, Wang Q, Han T, Weare E, Fu G (2017). "Differential emotion attribution to neutral faces of own and other races". Cognition and Emotion. 31 (2): 360–368. doi:10.1080/02699931.2015.1092419. PMID 26465265. S2CID 24973774.
  4. ^ Gómez-Emilsson, Andrés; Percy, Chris (16 November 2023). "The heavy-tailed valence hypothesis: the human capacity for vast variation in pleasure/pain and how to test it". Frontiers in Psychology. 14. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1127221. PMC 10687198. PMID 38034319.
  5. ^ Kliemann, Dorit; Jacoby, Nir; Anzellotti, Stefano; Saxe, Rebecca R. (2016-11-16). "Decoding task and stimulus representations in face-responsive cortex". Cognitive Neuropsychology. 33 (7–8): 362–377. doi:10.1080/02643294.2016.1256873. ISSN 0264-3294. PMC 5673491. PMID 27978778.
  6. ^ "Negative Valence Systems". National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH). Retrieved 2023-07-25.
  7. ^ "Positive Valence Systems". National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH). Retrieved 2023-07-25.