Jump to content

User:Leanna321/Rumination (psychology)

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Article Draft

[edit]

Lead

[edit]

Rumination is the focused attention on the symptoms of one's distress, and on its possible causes and consequences, as opposed to its solutions, according to the Response Styles Theory proposed by Nolen-Hoeksema (1998). Because the Response Styles Theory has been empirically supported, this model of rumination is the most widely used conceptualization. Other theories, however, have proposed different definitions for rumination. For example, in the Goal Progress Theory, rumination is conceptualized not as a reaction to a mood state, but as a "response to failure to progress satisfactorily towards a goal."

Another noteworthy model, the Goal Progress Theory, conceptualizes rumination as, not a reaction to a mood state, but as a "response to failure to progress satisfactorily towards a goal." As such, both rumination and worry are associated with anxiety and other negative emotional states; however, its measures have not been unified. Multiple tools exist to measure ruminative thoughts. Treatments specifically addressing ruminative thought patterns are still in the early stages of development.[1]

Types and content of thoughts

[edit]

Theories of rumination differ in their predictions regarding the content of ruminative thoughts based on their respective conceptualizations. Some models propose that rumination is focused on negative feeling states and/or the circumstances surrounding that emotion (RST, rumination on sadness, Trapnell and Campbell, stress-reactive rumination, post-event processing models). Rumination in other models focuses on discrepancies between one's current and desired status (goal progress, conceptual evaluative model of rumination). Finally, other models propose that it is the negative themes of uncontrollability and harm in metacognitions that are most important. Some common thoughts that are characteristic of ruminative responses are questioning the well-being of oneself and focusing on the possible causes and consequences of one's depressive symptoms (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). For example, some ruminative thoughts include "I'm in such a bad mood," "I just don't feel like doing anything," or "why am I such a loser?"

Three forms of rumination have been proposed:

  • State rumination, which involves dwelling on the consequences and feelings associated with the failure. State rumination is more common in people who are pessimistic, neurotic, and who have negative attributional styles.
  • Action rumination, which consists of task-oriented thought processes focused on goal-achievement and correction of mistakes.
  • Task-irrelevant rumination, which utilizes events or people unassociated with the blocked goal to distract a person from the failure.

Pathology

[edit]

Extensive research on the effects of rumination, or the tendency to self-reflect, shows that the negative form of rumination (associated with dysphoria) interferes with people's ability to focus on problem-solving and results in dwelling on negative thoughts about past failures. Evidence from studies suggests that the negative implications of rumination are due to cognitive biases, such as memory and attentional biases, which predispose ruminators to selectively devote attention to negative stimuli.

The organic causes of rumination are not fully understood. Research has identified the activation of certain regions in the brain's default mode networks as neural substrates of rumination, but the number of brain-imaging studies on rumination is limited.[2]

The tendency to negatively ruminate is a stable constant over time and serves as a significant risk factor for clinical depression. Not only are habitual ruminators more likely to become depressed, but experimental studies have demonstrated that people who are induced to ruminate experience greater depressed mood. There is also evidence that rumination is linked to general anxiety, post traumatic stress, binge drinking, eating disorders, and self-injurious behavior. Research suggests that rumination is somewhat associated with a higher frequency of non-suicidal self-injury, and more heavily associated with a history of non-suicidal self injury.[3]

Rumination was originally believed to predict the duration of depressive symptoms. In other words, ruminating about problems was presumed to be a form of memory rehearsal which was believed to actually lengthen the experience of depression. The evidence now suggests that although rumination contributes to depression, it is not necessarily correlated with the duration of symptoms.

Research on the relationships between executive functions and rumination has yielded mixed results. Some studies have observed a negative correlation with two executive functioning abilities, set-shifting and inhibition, but the magnitudes of those correlations were unclear.[4][5] Another study observed only one relationship between rumination and an executive function, specifically the ability to discard past information from working memory.[6] Other studies, however, found no relationship between rumination and working memory.[4][5]

Measurement

[edit]

The tendency to ruminate can be assessed with the Ruminative Responses Scale of the Response Styles Questionnaire. On this measure, people are asked to indicate how often they engage in 22 ruminative thoughts or behaviors when they feel sad or blue. (this exact information repeats in the next section where it is more relevant)

There are multiple tools for measuring rumination. These include the following:

Ruminative responses scale

[edit]

The tendency to ruminate can be assessed with the Ruminative Responses Scale of the Response Styles Questionnaire. On this measure, people are asked to indicate how often they engage in 22 ruminative thoughts or behaviors when they feel sad or blue.

Rumination on sadness scale

[edit]

The Rumination On Sadness Scale is a self-report tool consisting of 13 items that uses the Likert Scale to measure rumination of sadness.

Repetitive thought/thinking questionnaire

[edit]

The 31-item Repetitive Thinking Questionnaire (RTQ) measures worry, rumination, and post-event processing with the purpose of controlling for effects associated with a psychological diagnosis or disorder. It includes two subscales, Repetitive Negative Thinking (RNT) and Absence of Repetitive Thinking (ART). RNT is associated with anxiety, depression, and other negative emotions as it influences metacognitive beliefs, cognitive avoidance strategies, and maladaptive thought control strategies. The ART subscale reflects the absence of those negative emotions associated with the RNT subscale, essentially measuring their opposites.[7]

Rumination-reflection scale

[edit]

The Rumination-Reflection Scale involves 24 items. Half of the questions look for adaptive reflective thought while the other half note self-rumination focus. This scale incorporates the Likert Scale.

Sex differences

[edit]

According to Susan Nolen-Hoeksema, women tend to ruminate when they are depressed, whereas men tend to distract themselves. This difference in response style was proposed to explain the higher rates of depression in women compared to men. Research has supported the theory that women have a greater likelihood to ruminate than men, but the magnitude of this difference is small.[8] The prediction that men are more likely to distract themselves has not been consistently supported in research.[9]

A meta-analysis was performed on both the sex differences in rumination of adults and the rumination subtypes "brooding" and "reflection." Studies show that women's chances of experiencing depressive symptoms or depression was twice that of men. The response styles theory (RST) suggests this may be due, to some extent, to higher rates of rumination in women. Brooding can be operationalized as continuous, passive, negative internalized thoughts. It is highly connected to worsening depression. Reflection is neutral, rather than negative, more active observation of self. In the meta-analysis, women showed statistically significant increases in levels of both brooding and reflection, supporting RST. Interestingly, there was a much smaller sex difference in reflection than brooding. The meta-analyses found similar results across multiple study designs.

Treatment

[edit]

Rumination-focused cognitive behavior therapy

[edit]

Recent studies have begun developing a type of cognitive behavioral therapy that focuses on rumination, but further research is still needed.[1] Rumination-focused cognitive behavior therapy aims to teach patients to recognize when they begin to ruminate and ultimately re-frame the way they view themselves.[10]

[edit]

Rumination has been confounded with other similar constructs that may overlap with it. Worry and negative automatic thoughts are two of them.

Worry

[edit]

Rumination appears closely related to worry. Some models consider rumination to be a type of worry (S-REF).[11] Worry has been identified as "a chain of thoughts and images, negatively affect-laden and relatively uncontrollable; it represents an attempt to engage in mental problem solving on an issue whose outcome is uncertain, but contains the possibility of one or more negative outcomes."

Worry is often studied in the context of generalized anxiety disorder, whereas rumination is often studied in the context of major depressive disorder. Because of the high comorbidity of these two conditions, more recent research is exploring the overlap of worry and rumination.[11] Rumination has also been compared to worry, and in some models, is considered a type of worry (S-REF). Many researchers have noted the high comorbidity of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and depression; over 60% of clients who present with symptoms of GAD also qualify for a diagnosis of major depressive disorder. This significant concurrence has led to an increasing literature on the overlap between rumination, which is often studied in the context of depression, and worry, which is often studied in the context of GAD.[citation needed] (this was all copied straight from an article, so I reworded)

According to the Mental Health Foundation, rumination has been identified to be one of the main problems that leads to anxiety and depression. A study conducted by psychologists from the University of Liverpool, suggests that dwelling on negative events that have occurred in one's life is the biggest predictor of depression and anxiety.

Measures of rumination and worry have also demonstrated high correlations, above and beyond that of symptom measures of anxiety and depression (r=.66).[12](<--- added this wiki citation) Rumination and worry overlap in their relationships to anxiety and depression, although some studies do indicate specificity of rumination to depression and worry to anxiety. Rumination has been found to predict changes in both depression and anxiety symptoms and individuals with major depression have been reported to engage in levels of worry similar to individuals with GAD. As a whole, these studies suggest that rumination and worry are related not only to each other, but also each is related to symptoms of both depression and anxiety.

Other studies have demonstrated that the content of worry and rumination are distinct; worry thoughts are often focused on problem-solving and have a future orientation, whereas ruminative thoughts concern themes of loss and are more focused on the past. Rumination, as compared to worry, has also been associated with less effort and less confidence in problem solving.[13] It has also been suggested that rumination and worry serve different purposes, namely that rumination is associated with greater belief in the personal relevance of a situation and a larger need to understand it, whereas worry is associated with a desire to avoid worry thoughts (Watkins 2004b). Worry has also been hypothesized to contain more imagery than rumination; however, support for this has been mixed.

Overall, these studies suggest that worry and rumination are related constructs that both lead to depression and anxiety. It is likely that rumination and worry, as with rumination and reflection, are related types of repetitive negative thinking that may be better captured as subtypes of some larger construct, such as avoidant coping strategies.

Healthy self-disclosure

[edit]

Although rumination is generally unhealthy and associated with depression, thinking and talking about one's feelings can be beneficial under the right conditions. According to Pennebaker, healthy self-disclosure can reduce distress and rumination when it leads to greater insight and understanding about the source of one's problems. Thus, when people share their feelings with others in the context of supportive relationships, they are likely to experience growth. In contrast, when people repetitively ruminate and dwell on the same problem without making progress, they are likely to experience depression. Co-rumination is a process defined as "excessively discussing personal problems within a dyadic relationship", a construct that is relatively understudied in both its negative and positive trade-offs.

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b Watkins, Ed; Scott, Jan; Wingrove, Janet; Rimes, Katharine; Bathurst, Neil; Steiner, Herbert; Kennell–Webb, Sandra; Moulds, Michelle; Malliaris, Yanni (2007). "Rumination-focused cognitive behaviour therapy for residual depression: A case series". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 45 (9): 2144–2154. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.09.018. ISSN 0005-7967.
  2. ^ Zhou, Hui-Xia; Chen, Xiao; Shen, Yang-Qian; Li, Le; Chen, Ning-Xuan; Zhu, Zhi-Chen; Castellanos, Francisco Xavier; Yan, Chao-Gan (2020). "Rumination and the default mode network: Meta-analysis of brain imaging studies and implications for depression". NeuroImage. 206: 116287. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116287. ISSN 1053-8119.
  3. ^ Coleman, Sophie E.; Dunlop, Brendan J.; Hartley, Samantha; Taylor, Peter J. (June 2022). "The relationship between rumination and NSSI: A systematic review and meta‐analysis". British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 61 (2): 405–443. doi:10.1111/bjc.12350. ISSN 0144-6657.
  4. ^ a b Yang, Yingkai; Cao, Songfeng; Shields, Grant S.; Teng, Zhaojun; Liu, Yanling (2016-07-05). "The relationships between rumination and core executive functions: A meta-analysis". Depression and Anxiety. 34 (1): 37–50. doi:10.1002/da.22539. ISSN 1091-4269.
  5. ^ a b Vălenaș, Sergiu P.; Szentágotai-Tătar, Aurora (2017-09-01). "The relationship between rumination and executive functions: A meta-analysis". Journal of Evidence-Based Psychotherapies. 17 (2): 23–52. doi:10.24193/jebp.2017.2.2. ISSN 2360-0853.
  6. ^ Zetsche, Ulrike; Bürkner, Paul - Christian; Schulze, Lars (2018-06-07). "Shedding light on the association between repetitive negative thinking and deficits in cognitive control – a meta-analysis". dx.doi.org. Retrieved 2022-09-15.
  7. ^ McEvoy, Peter M.; Mahoney, Alison E.J.; Moulds, Michelle L. (2010). "Are worry, rumination, and post-event processing one and the same?". Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 24 (5): 509–519. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.03.008. ISSN 0887-6185.
  8. ^ Johnson, Daniel P.; Whisman, Mark A. (2013). "Gender differences in rumination: A meta-analysis". Personality and Individual Differences. 55 (4): 367–374. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.03.019. ISSN 0191-8869.
  9. ^ Strauss, J; Muday, T; McNall, K; Wong, M (1997). "Response Style Theory revisited: Gender differences and stereotypes in rumination and distraction". Sex Roles. 36: 771–792 – via Springer Link.
  10. ^ Feldhaus, Claudia G.; Jacobs, Rachel H.; Watkins, Edward R.; Peters, Amy T.; Bessette, Katie L.; Langenecker, Scott A. (2020-04-28). "Rumination-Focused Cognitive Behavioral Therapy Decreases Anxiety and Increases Behavioral Activation Among Remitted Adolescents". Journal of Child and Family Studies. 29 (7): 1982–1991. doi:10.1007/s10826-020-01711-7. ISSN 1062-1024.
  11. ^ a b Smith, Jeannette M.; Alloy, Lauren B. (2009). "A roadmap to rumination: A review of the definition, assessment, and conceptualization of this multifaceted construct". Clinical Psychology Review. 29 (2): 116–128. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.10.003. ISSN 0272-7358.
  12. ^ Beck, R.; Perkins, T. S. (2001). "Cognitive Content-Specificity for Anxiety and Depression: A Meta-Analysis". Cognitive Therapy and Research. 25 (6): 651–663 – via SpringerLink. {{cite journal}}: line feed character in |title= at position 42 (help)
  13. ^ Papageorgiou, Costas; Wells, Adrian, "Nature, Functions, and Beliefs about Depressive Rumination", Depressive Rumination, Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd, pp. 1–20, retrieved 2022-10-07

Peer review edit summary (just in case I posted it in the wrong sandbox...)