User:Halabasyouni1/sandbox
This is a user sandbox of Halabasyouni1. You can use it for testing or practicing edits. This is not the sandbox where you should draft your assigned article for a dashboard.wikiedu.org course. To find the right sandbox for your assignment, visit your Dashboard course page and follow the Sandbox Draft link for your assigned article in the My Articles section. |
Larissa Samuelson | |
---|---|
Nationality | American |
Alma mater | Indiana University |
Occupation(s) | Professor of Psychology, University of East Anglia |
Awards | APA Distinguished Scientific Early Career Contributions to Psychology |
Larissa Samuelson is an American psychologist known for her exploration in the fields of word learning, cognitive development, and the use of dynamic systems as a framework for understanding the developmental process.[1] She is Professor at the School of Psychology of the University of East Anglia.
Samuelson was the recipient of the 2010 Award for Distinguished Scientific Early Career Contributions to Psychology (area: Developmental Psychology) from the American Psychological Association.[2][3]
Biography
[edit]Dr. Samuelson received a Bachelors of Science with honors in 1993 and a joint Ph.D in Psychology and Cognitive Science from Indiana University in 2000, where she worked under the supervision of Linda B. Smith. From 2000 to 2015, she was faculty of the Department of Psychology at the University of Iowa. She and her husband, John Spencer, joined the faculty of School of Psychology at the University of East Anglia in 2015. Samuelson's research has been funded by grants from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.[4][5]
Research
[edit]Larissa Samuelson and John Spencer have developed a dynamic neural field model of infant visual exploration and word learning that provides a process-based account of how children associate names with objects in the visual environment.[6][7] Samuelson and her associates have conducted numerous studies of word learning in toddlers examining the basis for children's extensions of nouns to novel referents. These and earlier studies suggest that toddlers operate with a "shape bias" that prompts them to generalize usage of count nouns to other objects of the same shape,[8] [9] for example by referring to a zebra as a horse. Samuelson and her colleagues claim that children are "biased to attend to the shape of solid rigid objects when learning novel names."[10] This association between count nouns and shape is learned as children develop their early noun vocabularies, resulting in their attention being drawn to shape as a common organizing principle.[11][12]
Samuelson's lab has examined conditions under which children will generalize usage of a noun based on various features in addition to shape, such as material.[13] In one of her studies,[14] Samuelson and her colleagues gave 16-month-olds a variety of substances, including applesauce, pudding, soup and juice. Some of the toddlers got to play with the food in their high chairs whereas others sat at a table. When presenting each substance, the researchers named it using a made-up word, such as dax or kiv. After allowing the child a minute to explore the substance, the researchers showed them the same food in a different container and asked them to say its "name" (i.e., the made-up word). Children tested in the highchair demonstrated better recall of the names of the substances. Their results support the view that toddlers' learning of words is contextually bound; that is, toddlers benefit from learning new words for substances in a context that they already associate with food. [15] [16]
Representative Publications
[edit]- Horst, J. S., & Samuelson, L. K. (2008). Fast mapping but poor retention by 24-month-old infants. Infancy, 13(2), 128-157.
- Samuelson, L., & Smith, L. B. (2000). Grounding development in cognitive processes. Child Development, 71(1), 98-106.
- Samuelson, L. K., Smith, L. B., Perry, L. K., & Spencer, J. P. (2011). Grounding word learning in space. PloS one, 6(12), e28095.
- Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S., Landau, B., Gershkoff-Stowe, L., & Samuelson, L. (2002). Object name learning provides on-the-job training for attention. Psychological Science, 13(1), 13-19.
- Spencer, J. P., Blumberg, M. S., McMurray, B., Robinson, S. R., Samuelson, L. K., & Tomblin, J. B. (2009). Short arms and talking eggs: Why we should no longer abide the nativist–empiricist debate. Child Development Perspectives, 3(2), 79-87.
- Spencer, J. P., Perone, S., Smith, L. B., & Samuelson, L. K. (2011). Learning words in space and time: Probing the mechanisms behind the suspicious-coincidence effect. Psychological Science, 22(8), 1049-1057.
References
[edit]- ^ "Building biases: Word learning as a developmental cascade". American Psychological Association. Retrieved 2017-12-02.
- ^ "Larissa K. Samuelson: Award for distinguished scientific early career contributions to psychology". American Psychologist. 65 (8): 730–732. 2010. doi:10.1037/a0020441. PMID 21058769.
- ^ "2010 APA Distinguished Scientific Award Recipients". American Psychological Association.
- ^ "Professor Larissa Samuelson - UEA". University of East Anglila.
- ^ "Exposure to More Diverse Objects Speeds Word Learning in Tots". Association for Psychological Science. Retrieved 2017-12-02.
- ^ Samuelson, Larissa K.; Spencer, John P.; Jenkins, Gavin W.; Samuelson, Larissa K.; Spencer, John P.; Jenkins, Gavin W. (2013). "A Dynamic Neural Field Model of Word Learning". Theoretical and Computational Models of Word Learning. pp. 1–27. doi:10.4018/978-1-4666-2973-8.ch001. ISBN 9781466629738. Retrieved 2017-12-02.
- ^ "Nature? Nurture? Or simply neither? - Futurity". Futurity. 2009-07-23. Retrieved 2017-12-02.
- ^ Landau, Barbara; Smith, Linda B; Jones, Susan (1992). "Syntactic context and the shape bias in children's and adults' lexical learning". Journal of Memory and Language. 31 (6): 807–825. doi:10.1016/0749-596x(92)90040-5.
- ^ Landau, Barbara; Smith, Linda B.; Jones, Susan S. (1988). "The importance of shape in early lexical learning". Cognitive Development. 3 (3): 299–321. doi:10.1016/0885-2014(88)90014-7.
- ^ Samuelson, Larissa K.; Horst, Jessica S.; Schutte, Anne R.; Dobbertin, Brandi N. (2008). "Rigid thinking about deformables: do children sometimes overgeneralize the shape bias?". Journal of Child Language. 35 (3): 559–589. doi:10.1017/S0305000908008672. ISSN 1469-7602. PMC 3303162. PMID 18588715.
- ^ Perry, Lynn K.; Samuelson, Larissa K. (2011). "The Shape of the Vocabulary Predicts the Shape of the Bias". Frontiers in Psychology. 2: 345. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00345. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 3222225. PMID 22125547.
- ^ Smith, Linda B.; Jones, Susan S.; Landau, Barbara; Gershkoff-Stowe, Lisa; Samuelson, Larissa (2016-05-06). "Object name Learning Provides On-the-Job Training for Attention". Psychological Science. 13 (1): 13–19. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00403. PMID 11892773. S2CID 2869588.
- ^ Samuelson, Larissa K.; Horst, Jessica S. (2007-01-01). "Dynamic Noun Generalization: Moment-to-Moment Interactions Shape Children's Naming Biases". Infancy. 11 (1): 97–110. doi:10.1207/s15327078in1101_5. ISSN 1532-7078.
- ^ Perry, Lynn K.; Samuelson, Larissa K.; Burdinie, Johanna B. (2014-09-01). "Highchair philosophers: the impact of seating context-dependent exploration on children's naming biases". Developmental Science. 17 (5): 757–765. doi:10.1111/desc.12147. ISSN 1467-7687. PMC 4040339. PMID 24289734.
- ^ "High Chairs Help Toddlers Learn Messy Words". Live Science. Retrieved 2017-12-02.
- ^ M.D., Perri Klass. "To Smoosh Peas Is to Learn". New York Times. Retrieved 2017-12-02.