User:Chrisanchezz/sandbox
This is a user sandbox of Chrisanchezz. You can use it for testing or practicing edits. This is not the sandbox where you should draft your assigned article for a dashboard.wikiedu.org course. To find the right sandbox for your assignment, visit your Dashboard course page and follow the Sandbox Draft link for your assigned article in the My Articles section. |
My group and I will be re-constructing the Wikiepedia page on Oralism. We believe that there are a lot of great opportunities for this page to provide great insight on the topic of oralism without making it sound like a "story". Keeping in mind that oralism could lead to potential sources that are bias, we will be very cognizant of such articles and make sure to provide factual, un-bias and un-opinionated articles and sources. Some sources could include, but is not limited to, the use of research papers and various textbooks on the social structures and sides of oralism. We will be sure to include the history or oralism and its roots and where it currently stands today. Re-constructing this page will offer great context for folks who may not understand the meaning of oralism and its effects on the greater community. It truly will be a great resource for a clarification on the topic if done correctly.
***Original Article is copied here, we are editing throughout, but much of the original text is still present***
Oralism is the education of deaf students through oral language by using lip reading, speech, mimicking the mouth shapes and breathing patterns of speech[1] instead of using sign language within the classroom. Oralism came into popular use in the United States around the late 1860s. In 1867, The Clarke School for the Deaf in Northampton, Massachusetts was the first school to start teaching in this manner.[2] Oralism and its contrast; manualism, manifests itself in deaf education and has continued to spark controversy for involved communities.[3][4]
History
[edit]Early 16th Century Oralism
[edit]Oralism as the systematic education of deaf people began in Spain in the mid-1500s and was the byproduct of socioeconomic motives. The church barred deaf people from holy communion because they could not confess aloud. Deaf people were also prohibited from inheriting their family's wealth; therefore, to preserve the family wealth, deaf heirs in Spain were sent to Pedro Ponce de Leon after hearing that he taught a deaf man to talk in San Salvador Monastery in Oña.[5] Oralism provided members of the privileged classes with deaf children a way to channel their children's education and an opportunity to keep them away from the deaf community. Speaking had been equated with the higher classes and higher intellect, signing with the lower.[5]
Later 19th Century Oralism
[edit]Schools
[edit]Before the Clarke School for the Deaf made its in mark in deaf American education in the 1860s, there was a popular support of manualism.[6] Manual language soon became a less popular choice for deaf education due to the new Darwinist perspective.[6] Clarke School for the Deaf in 1867 became a "mainstream service" for deaf students through creating a "learn to listen" mentality.[2] This was done through the proper training of educators in auditory/oral education.[2]. Since its start, Clarke School for the Deaf has expanded and provided support to the oralism movement within deaf education and policy.[2]
Policy
[edit]In relation to the Early 16th Century Oralism in Spain, 19th century oralists viewed oral language as a superior form of communication.[7] Gardiner Green Hubbard[2], Horace Mann[7], Samuel Gridley Howe[7] and Alexander Graham Bell[8] were popular supporters of oralism and its impact on deaf education and services. Until the end of the 19th century, many educators of deaf America were deaf themselves.[3] However, oralists like Alexander Graham Bell began to wield increasing influence.[7][8] Bell and others believed in deaf assimilation with the mainstream hearing world.[8]
In 1878, International Congress on the Education of the Deaf (ICED) met in Paris to discuss the use of sign language and other issues within deaf education.[3] During the congregation, no Deaf members allowed to testify. In 1880, the International Congress on the Education of the Deaf (ICED) met again in Milan with 164 educators attending and one of them being deaf.[3] This meeting created the solely-oralist classroom preventing any form of sign language from being used.[3] After the Milan conference the deaf community refered to this time in history as "the dark ages for deaf education in America".[9][4]
Classroom (need to re-word to be less bias; could be used in "Efficacy in classroom")
[edit]Hearing educators who could not sign replaced deaf ones and, by mid-century eighty percent of American secondary schools for the deaf used the oral method exclusively.[10] Classes were conducted in an "unnatural" mixture of spoken and signed English. For example, "is" "was" and "the", which are not used in sign, were spelled out using the manual alphabet which is difficult to produce and comprehend.[10] Students were taught using the articulation method, which taught them how to speak and lip read.[5] Oralists believed that signs were no more than gross holistic gestures, which stood for English words in a one-to-one correspondence. Sentences in sign were thought to have no grammar. The facial expressions, such as exaggerated movements of the mouth, tongue, eyes, and lips, suggesting grimacing or excessive emotional display, triggered horror in hearing people. Students were asked to stop moving their faces when they signed, which is equivalent to asking hearing people to speak in declarative sentences uttered in monotone.[5]
20th Century
[edit]Movement towards manualism (need to re-word to be less bias)
[edit]Even though students were not allowed to use manual signs within the classroom, many of the deaf students preferred manual signs and used them frequently in their dorm rooms.[9] These early attempts at oralism were commonly criticized because of their starkness. Some deaf children were considered "oral failures" because they could not pick up oral language. Others thought that the techniques of oralism actually limited them on what they were taught because they always had to concentrate on the way the words were formed, not what they meant.[9][4]
Leaders of the manualist movement, including Edward M. Gallaudet, argued against the teaching of oralism because it restricted the ability of deaf students to communicate in what was considered their native language.[4] Moreover, "attempts to eliminate sign language were tantamount to stripping them of their identity, their community, and their culture".[4]
Policy Change
[edit]The retraction of laws forbidding the use of sign language in the classroom occurred in 2010 with the International Congress on the Education of the Deaf (ICED) in Vancouver[3]. Deaf Grassroots activists and the planning committee of ICED created a solution to provide proper education to the deaf globally[3].
Modern usage
[edit]Parental use of the oral approach typically stems from parental desire for their child to use a spoken language to communicate with the majority hearing population. They also feel the use of a spoken language will further their child’s literacy and written language skills in the classroom. The success of the oral approach in a classroom setting has not been fully evaluated.[11]
Oralist Schools and Their Mission
[edit]Clarke Schools for Hearing and Speech: A focus on helping deaf and hard of hearing children develop spoken English and listening skills. Their goal is to prepare students for the mianstream setting.
Mainstream Schooling
[edit]Efficacy
[edit]Education
[edit]One study compared the English development of deaf children with a cochlear implant versus what the English development might have been without the implant. English development was greater and more successful for the implanted deaf child than that of the non-implanted child based on the implementation of a predictive model. The predictive model employs age, residual hearing, and communication mode used by the child to predict the language development. Although deaf implanted children are already at a disadvantage for English development when compared to their hearing counterparts, the implant, on average, reduced what could have been an even larger deficit had the child not been implanted (based on the predictive model). The authors recommend implanting the child as early as possible.[12]
The studies did not consider how a non-implanted child exposed to a signed language and a bilingual/bicultural education could develop English skills in relation to a hearing child’s English development. Multiple studies find that by ensuring a deaf child has access to American Sign Language, their overall academic performance is better than those who are not.[13] In addition, one study finds that deaf children's vocabulary development paralleled their hearing counterpart's vocabulary development when they were exposed to a signed language. If deaf children do not have full, unhindered access to a language from birth, studies indicate that the development of a second language is likely to be delayed.[14]
Communication in oral-deaf students is typically less frequent and less complex than hearing peers of the same age. These expressed communications are less clear than that of their hearing peers.[15] Linguistically, these communications are typical of the language skills seen much earlier in their hearing counterparts.[15] Despite efforts to encourage the sole reliance on speech and spoken language in oral schools, many oral-deaf individuals tend to develop sign systems among themselves in non-supervised settings.[16] Additionally, oral-deaf children often use manual gestures/signs simultaneously or in addition to vocalizations during expressive communications at home.[15]
Some studies have called into question the role of developing spoken language skills in relation to developing reading skills. One study in particular demonstrated that while individuals who became deaf before developing spoken language did show a decreased ability to differentiate between the phonological properties of a language, they showed equal capability of recognizing and understanding the orthographic properties of what they were reading.[17] In fact, compared to their hearing counterparts, the deaf individuals showed an increased rate of written word processing skills as they increased in age. Altogether, this research provided evidence contrary to the belief that spoken skills are critical to the development of reading skills, and further proposes that educational approaches should include a stronger focus on building awareness of written language forms separate from the related aural aspects.[17]
There also is no accurate predictor of oralism's success in the classroom.[18]
Social
[edit]Deaf children growing up in an oral setting are likely to experience cognitive and developmental delays as a result of a deprivation of natural signed-language. This decision to place the child in an oral setting however likely stems from pressure parents feel to do so from a doctor or medical practitioner. Pressure is then said to be placed on Deaf children, as they are reported to feel embarrassed if they do not master both lip-reading and spoken English. These delays are also not isolated to simply academic ones, but also social ones. Deaf children in an oral setting may feel depressed, anxious and experience aloneness and embarrassment as a result of such language deprivation. [18]
See Also
References
[edit]- ^ Through Deaf Eyes. Diane Garey, Lawrence R. Hott. DVD, PBS (Direct), 2007.
- ^ a b c d e "Welcome to Clarke". Clarke School for Hearing and Speech. Retrieved March 12th, 2017.
{{cite web}}
: Check date values in:|access-date=
(help) - ^ a b c d e f g "21st International Congress on the Education of the Deaf (ICED) in July 2010 in Vancouver, Canada". World Federation of the Deaf. 2016. Retrieved March 12, 2017.
- ^ a b c d e Winefield, Richard. Never the Twain Shall Meet. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press, 1987. 4.
- ^ a b c d Cohen, Leah. Train Go Sorry. New York, New York: First Vintage Books, 1995.
- ^ a b Baynton, Douglas C. (1996). Forbidden Signs: American Culture and the Campaign Against Sign Language. United States of America: University of Chicago Press. pp. 36–56. ISBN 9780226039640.
- ^ a b c d 1949-, Winefield, Richard (1987-01-01). Never the twain shall meet : Bell, Gallaudet, and the communications debate. Gallaudet University Press. ISBN 9781563680564. OCLC 223261645.
{{cite book}}
:|last=
has numeric name (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c "Introduction: Sucesses and Challenges: Listening to their Voices". Alexander Graham Bell Association for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing. Retrieved March 12, 2017.
- ^ a b c Cite error: The named reference
Eyes
was invoked but never defined (see the help page). - ^ a b Fox, Margalit. Talking Hands. Simon & Schuster Paperbacks. New York, New York: 2007
- ^ Swanwick, Ruth (2012-12-06). Issues in Deaf Education. Routledge. ISBN 9781136619977.
- ^ "Redirection To The Login script" (PDF). www.jstor.org.ezproxy.bu.edu. JSTOR 40063514. Retrieved 2017-03-12.
- ^ "More Than Meets the Eye". gupress.gallaudet.edu. Retrieved 2017-03-12.
- ^ "An Investigation of Speechreading in Profoundly Congenitally Deaf British Adults" (PDF). Dissertation Publishing – via ProQuest.
- ^ a b c Nicholas, Johanna; Geers, Ann (1997). "Communication of oral deaf and normally hearing children at 36 months of age". Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 40 (6): 1314–1327. doi:10.1044/jslhr.4006.1314. PMID 9430751 – via ResearchGate.
- ^ Anglin-Jaffe, Hannah (2013-05-01). "Signs of Resistance: Peer Learning of Sign Languages Within 'Oral' Schools for the Deaf". Studies in Philosophy and Education. 32 (3): 261–271. doi:10.1007/s11217-012-9350-3. ISSN 0039-3746. S2CID 145422142.
- ^ a b Miller, Paul (2007-08-01). "The Role of Phonology in the Word Decoding Skills of Poor Readers: Evidence from Individuals with Prelingual Deafness or Diagnosed Dyslexia". Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities. 19 (4): 385–408. doi:10.1007/s10882-007-9057-5. ISSN 1056-263X. S2CID 141473313.
- ^ a b "SAGE Knowledge". sk.sagepub.com. Retrieved 2017-04-19.