Jump to content

User:Aamiller90/sandbox

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

History

[edit]

Earlier societies utilized elaborate methods of lie detection which mainly involved torture; for instance, the Middle Ages used boiling water to detect liars as it was believed honest men would withstand it better than liars.[1] Early devices for lie detection include an 1895 invention of Cesare Lombroso used to measure changes in blood pressure for police cases, a 1904 device by Vittorio Benussi used to measure breathing, and an abandoned project by American William Marston which used blood pressure to examine German prisoners of war (POWs).[2] Marston’s machine indicated a strong positive correlation between systolic blood pressure and lying.[3]

Marston wrote a second paper on the concept in 1915, when finishing his undergraduate studies. He entered Harvard Law School and graduated in 1918, re-publishing his earlier work in 1917.[4] Marston's main inspiration for the device was his wife, #Elizabeth Holloway Marston.[5] According to their son, Marston's wife, #Elizabeth Holloway Marston, was also involved in the development of the systolic blood pressure test: "According to Marston’s son, it was his mother Elizabeth, Marston’s wife, who suggested to him that 'When she got mad or excited, her blood pressure seemed to climb' (Lamb, 2001). Although Elizabeth is not listed as Marston’s collaborator in his early work, Lamb, Matte (1996), and others refer directly and indirectly to Elizabeth’s work on her husband’s deception research. She also appears in a picture taken in his polygraph laboratory in the 1920s (reproduced in Marston, 1938)."[6][7] The comic book character, Wonder Woman, by William Marston (and influenced by Elizabeth Marston[8][9]) carries a magic lasso modelled upon the pneumograph (breathing monitor) test.[8][10]

Despite his predecessor's contributions, Marston styled himself the “father of the polygraph” . Marston remained the device's primary advocate, lobbying for its use in the courts. In 1938 he published a book, The Lie Detector Test, wherein he documented the theory and use of the device.[11] In 1938 he appeared in advertising by the Gillette company claiming that the polygraph showed Gillette razors were better than the competition.[12][13][14]

A device recording both blood pressure and galvanic skin response was invented in 1921 by Dr. John Augustus Larson of the University of California and first applied in law enforcement work by the Berkeley Police Department under its nationally renowned police chief August Vollmer. Further work on this device was done by Leonarde Keeler.[15] As Larson's protege, Keeler updated the device by making it portable and added the galvanic skin response to it in 1939. His device was then purchased by the FBI, and served as the prototype of the modern polygraph. [16]

Several devices similar to Keeler's polygraph version included the Berkeley Psychograph, a blood pressure-pulse-respiration recorder developed by C. D. Lee in 1936[17] and the Darrow Behavior Research Photopolygraph, which was developed and intended solely for behavior research experiments.[17][18]

A device which recorded muscular activity accompanying changes in blood pressure was developed in 1945 by John E. Reid, who claimed that greater accuracy could be obtained by making these recordings simultaneously with standard blood pressure-pulse-respiration recordings.[17][19]

Testing procedure

[edit]


Today, polygraph examiners use two types of instrumentation: analog and computerized. In the United States, most examiners now use computerized instrumentation.[citation needed]

A typical polygraph test starts with a pre-test interview to gain some preliminary information which will later be used for "control questions", or CQ. Then the tester will explain how the polygraph is supposed to work, emphasizing that it can detect lies and that it is important to answer truthfully. Then a "stim test" is often conducted: the subject is asked to deliberately lie and then the tester reports that he was able to detect this lie. Guilty subjects are likely to become more anxious when they are reminded of the test's validity. However, there are risks of innocent subjects being equally or more anxious than the guilty.[20] Then the actual test starts. Some of the questions asked are "irrelevant" or IR ("Is your name Chris?"), others are "probable-lie" control questions that most people will lie about ("Have you ever stolen money?") and the remainder are the "relevant questions", or RQ, that the tester is really interested in. The different types of questions alternate. The test is passed if the physiological responses during the probable-lie control questions (CQ) are larger than those during the relevant questions (RQ). If this is not the case, the tester attempts to elicit admissions during a post-test interview, for example, "Your situation will only get worse if we don't clear this up".[21][22]

Criticisms have been given regarding the validity of the administration of the Comparative Questions test (CQT). The CQT may be vulnerable to being conducted in an interrogation-like fashion. This kind of interrogation style would elicit a nervous response from innocent and guilty suspects alike. There are several other ways of administrating the questions.

An alternative is the Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT), or the Concealed Information Test (CIT), which is being used in Japan.[23] The administration of this test is given to prevent potential errors that may arise from the questioning style. The test is usually conducted by a tester with no knowledge of the crime or circumstances in question. The administrator tests the participant on their knowledge of the crime that would not be known to an innocent person. For example: "Was the crime committed with a .45 or a 9 mm?" The questions are in multiple choice and the participant is rated on how they react to the correct answer. If they react strongly to the guilty information, then proponents of the test believe that it is likely that they know facts relevant to the case. This administration is considered more valid by supporters of the test because it contains many safeguards to avoid the risk of the administrator influencing the results.[24]

Validity

[edit]

Polygraphy has little credibility among scientists.[25][26] Despite claims of 90-95% validity by polygraph advocates, and 95-100% by businesses providing polygraph services,[non-primary source needed] critics maintain that rather than a "test", the method amounts to an inherently unstandardizable interrogation technique whose accuracy cannot be established. A 1997 survey of 421 psychologists estimated the test's average accuracy at about 61%, a little better than chance.[27] Critics also argue that even given high estimates of the polygraph's accuracy a significant number of subjects (e.g. 10% given a 90% accuracy) will appear to be lying, and would unfairly suffer the consequences of "failing" the polygraph. In the 1998 Supreme Court case, United States v. Scheffer, the majority stated that "There is simply no consensus that polygraph evidence is reliable" and "Unlike other expert witnesses who testify about factual matters outside the jurors' knowledge, such as the analysis of fingerprints, ballistics, or DNA found at a crime scene, a polygraph expert can supply the jury only with another opinion..."[28] Also, in 2005 the 11th Circuit Court of Appeals stated that “polygraphy did not enjoy general acceptance from the scientific community”.[29] Charles Honts, a psychology professor at Boise State University, states that polygraph interrogations give a high rate of false positives on innocent people.[30] In 2001 William G. Iacono, Distinguished McKnight University Professor of Psychology and Neuroscience and Director, Clinical Science and Psychopathology Research Training Program at the University of Minnesota, published a paper titled 'Forensic "Lie Detection": Procedures Without Scientific Basis' in the peer reviewed Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice. He concluded that

Although the CQT [Control Question Test] may be useful as an investigative aid and tool to induce confessions, it does not pass muster as a scientifically credible test. CQT theory is based on naive, implausible assumptions indicating (a) that it is biased against innocent individuals and (b) that it can be beaten simply by artificially augmenting responses to control questions. Although it is not possible to adequately assess the error rate of the CQT, both of these conclusions are supported by published research findings in the best social science journals (Honts et al., 1994; Horvath, 1977; Kleinmuntz & Szucko, 1984; Patrick & Iacono, 1991). Although defense attorneys often attempt to have the results of friendly CQTs admitted as evidence in court, there is no evidence supporting their validity and ample reason to doubt it. Members of scientific organizations who have the requisite background to evaluate the CQT are overwhelmingly skeptical of the claims made by polygraph proponents.

[31]

Summarizing the consensus in psychological research, professor David W. Martin, PhD, from North Carolina State University, states that people have tried to use the polygraph for measuring human emotions, but there is simply no royal road to (measuring) human emotions.[32] Therefore, since one cannot reliably measure human emotions (especially when one has an interest in hiding his/her emotions), the idea of valid detection of truth or falsehood through measuring respiratory rate, blood volume, pulse rate and galvanic skin response is a mere pretense. Since psychologists cannot ascertain what emotions one has,[33] polygraph professionals are not able to do that either.

Polygraphy has also been faulted for failing to trap known spies such as double-agent Aldrich Ames, who passed two polygraph tests while spying for the Soviet Union.[30][34] Other spies who passed the polygraph include Karl Koecher,[35] Ana Belen Montes,[36] and Leandro Aragoncillo.[37] However, CIA spy Harold James Nicholson failed his polygraph examinations, which aroused suspicions that led to his eventual arrest.[38] Polygraph examination and background checks failed to detect Nada Nadim Prouty, who was not a spy but was convicted for improperly obtaining US citizenship and using it to obtain a restricted position at the FBI.[39]

The polygraph also failed to catch Gary Ridgway, the "Green River Killer". Another suspect named Melvin Foster allegedly failed a given lie detector test whereas Ridgeway passed.[40] Ridgway passed a polygraph in 1984 and confessed almost 20 years later when confronted with DNA evidence.[41]

Conversely, innocent people have been known to fail polygraph tests. In Wichita, Kansas in 1986, after failing two polygraph tests (one police administered, the other given by an expert that he had hired), Bill Wegerle had to live under a cloud of suspicion of murdering his wife Vicki Wegerle, even though he was neither arrested nor convicted of her death. In March 2004, a letter was sent to The Wichita Eagle reporter Hurst Laviana that contained Vicki's drivers license and what first appeared to be crime scene photographs of her body. The photos had actually been taken by her true murderer, BTK,[42] the serial killer that had plagued the people of Wichita since 1974 and had recently resurfaced in February 2004 after an apparent 25 year period of dormancy (he had actually killed three women between 1985 and 1991, including Wegerle). That effectively cleared Bill Wegerle of the murder of his wife. In 2005 conclusive DNA evidence, including DNA retrieved from under the fingernails of Vicki Wegerle, demonstrated that the BTK Killer was Dennis Rader.[43]

Prolonged polygraph examinations are sometimes used as a tool by which confessions are extracted from a defendant, as in the case of Richard Miller, who was persuaded to confess largely by polygraph results combined with appeals from a religious leader.[44]

In the high-profile disappearance of 7-year-old Danielle van Dam of San Diego in 2002, neighbor David Westerfield became the prime suspect when he failed his polygraph with a greater than 99% probability he was lying.[45] But this claim has been challenged because the examiner kept the space heater on, making Westerfield uncomfortably hot; he constantly adjusted his machine, he said because the readings were too low; and he made a number of subtle changes to the questions he said he was going to ask, even though he said he wouldn’t.[46] The examiner thought Westerfield’s alibi, a meandering weekend trip, was "as crazy as it gets"; he interviewed him for 3 hours, and at the end of the test, accused him of being involved in Danielle’s disappearance.[47] Westerfield attributed his failing the test to his compassion for Danielle's family.[48] In subsequent letters he said that, a couple of days later, they discovered that his test contained false results and they asked him to retake the test, but his attorney, who said it probably meant that the first test wasn’t set up right, wouldn’t allow it.[49]

Law enforcement agencies and intelligence agencies in the United States are by far the biggest users of polygraph technology. In the United States alone all federal law enforcement agencies either employ their own polygraph examiners or use the services of examiners employed in other agencies.[50] This is despite their unreliability. For example in 1978 Richard Helms, the 8th Director of Central Intelligence, stated that:

"We discovered there were some Eastern Europeans who could defeat the polygraph at any time. Americans are not very good at it, because we are raised to tell the truth and when we lie it is easy to tell we are lying. But we find a lot of Europeans and Asiatics [who] can handle that polygraph without a blip, and you know they are lying and you have evidence that they are lying."[51]

Many people misunderstand that the polygraph can detect lies; lies are not measurable variables. Polygraphs actually measure physiological arousal which can pinpoint deception. When used properly, supporters assert that the polygraph is highly accurate.[52]

Alternative Tests

[edit]

Most polygraph researchers have focused more the exam's predictive value on a subject’s guilt. However, there have been no empirical theories established to explain how a polygraph measures deception. Recent research indicates that Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) may benefit in explaining the psychological correlations of polygraph exams. It could also explain which parts of the brain are active when subjects use artificial memories.[53] Most brain activity occurs in both sides of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, which is linked to response inhibition. This indicates that deception involves a subject’s inhibition of certain questions, such as attempting to be truthful while lying. Recalling artificial memories are known to activate the posterior cingulate cortex. However, fMRIs are limited to being expensive, immobile, and having inconsistent lying responses. Some researchers believe that reaction time (RT) based tests may replace polygraphs in concealed information detection. RT based tests differ from polygraphs in stimulus presentation duration, and can be conducted without physiological recording as subject response time is measured via computer. However, researchers have found limitations to these tests as subjects voluntarily control their reaction time, deception can still occur within the response deadline, and the test itself lacks physiological recording.[54]

Notes

[edit]
  1. ^ Grubin, D., & Madsen, L. (2005). Lie detection and the polygraph: A historical review. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, 16(2), 357-369.
  2. ^ Nitv Llc
  3. ^ Lewis, J. A., & Cuppari, M. (2009). The polygraph: The truth lies within. Journal of Psychiatry and Law, 37(1), 85-92.
  4. ^ Marston, William M. "Systolic Blood Pressure Changes in Deception," Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2:117-163.
  5. ^ Grubin, D., & Madsen, L. (2005). Lie detection and the polygraph: A historical review. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, 16(2), 357-369.
  6. ^ WILLIAM MOULTON MARSTON, THE NATIONAL RESEARCH COUNCIL, AND WONDER WOMAN
  7. ^ The Polygraph and Lie Detection
  8. ^ a b Who Was Wonder Woman? Long-ago LAW alumna Elizabeth Marston was the muse who gave us a superheroine
  9. ^ OUR TOWNS; She's Behind the Match For That Man of Steel
  10. ^ The Polygraph and Lie Detection, p.295
  11. ^ Marston, William Moulton. The Lie Detector Test. New York: Richard R. Smith, 1938.
  12. ^ "William Marston's Secret Identity". Reason magazine. May 2001.
  13. ^ Now! Lie Detector Charts Emotional Effects of Shaving - 1938 Gillette Advertisement
  14. ^ FBI File of William Moulton Marston (including report on Gillette advertising campaign)
  15. ^ Leonarde Keeler and his Instruments
  16. ^ Grubin, D., & Madsen, L. (2005). Lie detection and the polygraph: A historical review. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, 16(2), 357-369
  17. ^ a b c Inbau, Fred E. Lie Detection and Criminal Interrogation, The Williams & Wilkins Company, 1948
  18. ^ Troville, P. V. "A History of Lie Detection," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 29 (6)-848 (1939); 30 (1):104 (1939)
  19. ^ Reid, J. E. "Simulated Blood Pressure Responses in Lie-Detection Tests and a Method for Their Deception," Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 36 (1):201-215 (1945)
  20. ^ Lewis, J. A., & Cuppari, M. (2009). The polygraph: The truth lies within. Journal of Psychiatry and Law, 37(1), 85-92
  21. ^ For details on the intricacies of various polygraph techniques, see the Federal Psychophysiological Detection of Deception Examiner Handbook, the U.S. Government's official guide to the administration of polygraph examinations.
  22. ^ For interrogation techniques associated with polygraph testing, see the Department of Defense Polygraph Institute's Interview & Interrogation Handbook.
  23. ^ Don Sosunov (October 14, 2010). "The Admissibility of Polygraph Evidence in Criminal Courts".{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  24. ^ For more info on the GKT, see the The Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT) as an Application of Psychophysiology: Future Prospects and Obstacles.
  25. ^ "Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation". Washington, D. C.: U.S. Congress Office of Technology Assessment. 1983. Retrieved 2008-02-29.
  26. ^ "Monitor on Psychology - The polygraph in doubt". American Psychological Association. 07-2004. Retrieved 2008-02-29. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  27. ^ Vergano, Dan (2002). "Telling the truth about lie detectors". USA Today. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  28. ^ "United States v. Scheffer". 1998. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  29. ^ "United States v. Henderson" (PDF). 2005. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help) [PDF]
  30. ^ a b Hess, Pamela, "Pentagon's Intelligence Arm Steps Up Lie-Detector Efforts", Arizona Daily Star, August 24, 2008. Also in Fox News via AP [1]
  31. ^ William G. Iacono (2001). "Forensic "Lie Detection": Procedures Without Scientific Basis" (PDF). Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice.
  32. ^ Psychology of Human Behavior The Teaching Company, course No. 1620 taught by David W. Martin, lesson 19.
  33. ^ Professor Martin considers an urban legend the idea that psychologists could read people's minds or look inside their heads and know their thoughts/emotions, cf. lesson 1 of the same course.
  34. ^ Ames provides personal insight into the U.S. Government's reliance on polygraphy in a 2000 letter to Steven Aftergood of the Federation of American Scientists.Ames, Aldrich (2000). "A Letter from Aldrich Ames on Polygraph Testing". Federation of American Scientists. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  35. ^ Kessler, Ron. "Moscow's Mole in the CIA: How a Swinging Czech Superspy Stole America's Most Sensitive Secrets," Washington Post, April 17, 1988, C1.
  36. ^ Bachelet, Pablo (October 13, 2006). "Book outlines how spy exposed U.S. intelligence secrets to Cuba". McClatchey Washington Bureau. "She first came under U.S. suspicion in 1994, when Cuba detected a highly secret electronic surveillance system. Montes took a polygraph test and passed it."
  37. ^ Ross, Brian and Richard Esposito (October 6, 2005). "Investigation Continues: Security Breach at the White House". ABC News. "Officials say Aragoncillo passed several lie detector tests that are routinely given to individuals with top secret clearances."
  38. ^ "Dept. of Energy, Office of Counterintelligence". Harold James Nicholson Dossier. Retrieved 2009-06-01.
  39. ^ Warrick, Joby; Eggen, Dan (2007-11-14). "Ex-FBI Employee's Case Raises New Security Concerns Sham Marriage Led to U.S. Citizenship". Washington Post. Retrieved May 22, 2010.
  40. ^ Lewis, J. A., & Cuppari, M. (2009). The polygraph: The truth lies within. Journal of Psychiatry and Law, 37(1), 85-92.
  41. ^ "What made Ridgway kill still a riddle". The News Tribune. 2003-11-09.
  42. ^ www.harpercollins.com
  43. ^ Oct. 1, 2005 48 Hours Mysteries: BTK: Out Of The Shadows
  44. ^ "United States of America versus William Galbreth" (PDF). 1995-03-09.
  45. ^ Roth, Alex. “Westerfield failed polygraph test badly: 'Greater than 99%' chance he was lying, examiner says on tape,” San Diego Union-Tribune, January 9, 2003.
  46. ^ Stevenson, C. “Rush to Judgement,” CreateSpace, June 22, 2011, pages 180-184.
  47. ^ Green, Kristen. “Police were on to killer quickly: Investigators say first few days key to cracking case,” San Diego Union-Tribune, September 18, 2002.
  48. ^ Ryan, Harriet. “Lie detected, Westerfield claims compassion,” Court TV, January 9, 2003.
  49. ^ Roth, Alex.“Westerfield's death-row letters: Killer claims he was framed for Danielle van Dam murder,” San Diego Union-Tribune, May 3, 2003.
  50. ^ url=http://www.nettrace.com.au/content/nta10001.htm
  51. ^ url=http://mcadams.posc.mu.edu/russ/jfkinfo2/jfk4/Hscahelm.htm
  52. ^ Lewis, J. A., & Cuppari, M. (2009). The polygraph: The truth lies within. Journal of Psychiatry and Law, 37(1), 85-92
  53. ^ Bell, B. G., & Grubin, D. (2010). Functional magnetic resonance imaging may promote theoretical understanding of the polygraph test. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, 21(1), 52-65
  54. ^ Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., Degrootte, T., & Rosseel, Y. (2010). Detecting concealed information with reaction times: Validity and comparison with the polygraph. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(7), 991-100.


User:Aamiller90/sandbox