Jump to content

Stanislas Dehaene

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Stanislas Dehaene
Stanislas Dehaene in 2014
Born (1965-05-12) 12 May 1965 (age 59)
Roubaix, France[1]
Alma materÉcole normale supérieure
University of Paris VI
Known forNumerical cognition, Neural correlates of reading and consciousness
AwardsJames S. McDonnell Foundation "Genius Award", Louis D. Prize, Prix Jean Rostand (for La Bosse des Maths)
Scientific career
FieldsCognitive Neuroscience
InstitutionsINSERM Unit 562 "Cognitive Neuroimaging" (director); Collège de France (professor)
Doctoral advisorJacques Mehler

Stanislas Dehaene (born May 12, 1965) is a French author and cognitive neuroscientist whose research centers on a number of topics, including numerical cognition, the neural basis of reading and the neural correlates of consciousness. As of 2017, he is a professor at the Collège de France and, since 1989, the director of INSERM Unit 562, "Cognitive Neuroimaging".[2]

Dehaene was one of ten people to be awarded the James S. McDonnell Foundation Centennial Fellowship[3] in 1999 for his work on the "Cognitive Neuroscience of Numeracy". In 2003, together with Denis Le Bihan, Dehaene was awarded the Grand Prix scientifique de la Fondation Louis D. from the Institut de France.[4] He was elected to the American Philosophical Society in 2010.[5] In 2014, together with Giacomo Rizzolatti and Trevor Robbins, he was awarded the Brain Prize.[6]

Dehaene is an associate editor of the journal Cognition, and a member of the editorial board of several other journals, including NeuroImage, PLoS Biology, Developmental Science, and Neuroscience of Consciousness.[7]

Early life and education

[edit]

Dehaene studied mathematics at the École Normale Supérieure in Paris from 1984 to 1989.[1] He obtained his master's degree in Applied mathematics and computer science in 1985 from the University of Paris VI.[1]

He turned to neuroscience and psychology[when?] after reading Jean-Pierre Changeux's book, L'Homme neuronal (Neuronal Man: The Biology of The Mind).[citation needed]

Dehaene began to collaborate on computational neuronal models of human cognition, including working memory and task control, collaborations which continue to the present day.[1] Dehaene completed his PhD in Experimental Psychology in 1989 with Jacques Mehler at the École des Hautes Études en Sciences Sociales (EHESS), Paris.[1]

Career

[edit]

After receiving his doctorate, Dehaene became a research scientist at INSERM in the Cognitive Sciences and Psycholinguistics Laboratory (Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique) directed by Mehler.[1] He spent two years, from 1992 to 1994, as a post-doctoral fellow at the Institute of Cognitive and Decision Sciences, with Michael Posner at the University of Oregon.[1]

Dehaene returned to France in 1997[8] to serve as Research Director at INSERM (French National Institute of Health and Medical Research) through 2005. He subsequently began his own research group, which today[when?] numbers nearly 30 graduate students, post-doctoral fellows and researchers.[1] In 2005, he was elected to the newly created Chair of Experimental Cognitive Psychology at the Collège de France.[1]

Work

[edit]

Numerical cognition

[edit]

Dehaene is best known for his work on numerical cognition, a discipline which he popularized and synthesized with the publication of his 1997 book, The Number Sense (La Bosse des maths) which won the Prix Jean-Rostand [fr] for best French language general-audience scientific book. He began his studies of numerical cognition with Jacques Mehler, examining the cross-linguistic frequency of number words,[9] whether numbers were understood in an analog or compositional manner,[10][11] and the connection between numbers and space (the "SNARC effect").[12] With Changeux, he then developed a computational model of numerical abilities, which predicted log-gaussian tuning functions for number neurons,[13] a finding which has now been elegantly confirmed with single-unit physiology[14]

With long-time collaborator Laurent Cohen, a neurologist at the Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital in Paris, Dehaene also identified patients with lesions in different regions of the parietal lobe with impaired multiplication, but preserved subtraction (associated with lesions of the inferior parietal lobule) and others with impaired subtraction, but preserved multiplication (associated with lesions to the intraparietal sulcus).[15] This double dissociation suggested that different neural substrates for overlearned, linguistically mediated calculations, like multiplication, are mediated by inferior parietal regions, while on-line computations, like subtraction are mediated by the intraparietal sulcus. Shortly thereafter, Dehaene began EEG[16][17] and functional neuroimaging[18][19][20] studies of these capacities, showing that parietal and frontal regions were specifically involved in mathematical cognition, including the dissociation between subtraction and multiplication observed in his previous patient studies.

Together with Pierre Pica, and Elizabeth Spelke, Stanislas Dehaene has studied the numeracy and numeral expressions of the Mundurucu (an indigenous tribe living in Para, Brazil).[21]

Consciousness

[edit]

Dehaene subsequently turned his attention to work on the neural correlates of consciousness, leading to numerous scientific articles, an edited book, "The Cognitive Neuroscience of Consciousness" and is the Past President of the Association for the Scientific Study of Consciousness. Dehaene has developed computational models of consciousness, based on Bernard Baars's Global Workspace Theory, which suggest that only one piece of information can gain access to a "global neuronal workspace".[22] To explore the neural basis of this global neuronal workspace, he has conducted functional neuroimaging experiments of masking and the attentional blink, which show that information that reaches conscious awareness leads to increased activation in a network of parietal and frontal regions.[23][24][25] However, some of his work on this subject has been called into question due to a methodological flaw in the "standard reasoning of unconscious priming".[26]

Neural basis of reading

[edit]

In addition, Dehaene has used brain imaging to study language processing in monolingual and bilingual subjects, and in collaboration with Laurent Cohen, the neural basis of reading. Dehaene and Cohen initially focused on the role of ventral stream regions in visual word recognition, and in particular the role of the left inferior temporal cortex for reading written words. They identified a region they called the "visual word form area" (VWFA) that was consistently activated during reading,[27][28][29] and also found that when this region was surgically removed to treat patients with intractable epilepsy, reading abilities were severely impaired.[30]

Dehaene, Cohen and colleagues have subsequently demonstrated that, rather than being a single area, the VWFA is the highest stage in a hierarchy of visual feature extraction for letter and word recognition.[31][32]

More recently, they have turned their attention to how learning to read may depend on a process of "neuronal recycling" that causes brain circuits originally evolved for object recognition to become tuned to recognize frequent letters, pairs of letters and words,[33] and have tested these ideas examining brain responses in a group of adults who did not learn to read due to social and cultural constraints.[34][35]

Bibliography

[edit]

As editor

[edit]
  • Dehaene, S. (Ed.) Numerical Cognition. Oxford, Blackwell. ISBN 1-55786-444-6.
  • Dehaene, S. (Ed.) Le Cerveau en action: l'imagerie cérébrale en psychologie cognitive. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, 1997. ISBN 2-13-048270-8.
  • Dehaene, S. (Ed.) The Cognitive Neuroscience of Consciousness. MIT Press, 2001. ISBN 0-262-54131-9.
  • Dehaene, S. Duhamel, J.R., Hauser, M. and Rizzolatti, G. (Ed.) From Monkey Brain to Human Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2005. ISBN 0-262-04223-1.

As author

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b c d e f g h i Curriculum Vitae. unicog.org. Last updated Monday, 30 August 2010
  2. ^ "Welcome to the INSERM-CEA Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit". unicog.org. 27 January 2013. Retrieved 18 February 2013.
  3. ^ "James S. McDonnell Foundation". Jsmf.org. Retrieved 18 February 2013.
  4. ^ "Louis D. Prize" (in French). Institut de France. 2003. Archived from the original on 27 September 2011.
  5. ^ "APS Member History". search.amphilsoc.org. Retrieved 21 April 2021.
  6. ^ "Biography Stanislas Dehaene". thebrainprize.org.
  7. ^ "Neuroscience of Consciousness". nc.oxfordjournals.org. Oxford University Press. Archived from the original on 23 January 2015. Retrieved 26 January 2015.
  8. ^ Stanislas Dehaene Curriculum Vitae, last updated Monday, 13 February, 2017.
  9. ^ Dehaene S.; Mehler J. (1992). "Cross-linguistic regularities in the frequency of number words". Cognition. 43 (1): 1–29. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(92)90030-l. PMID 1591901. S2CID 30060533.
  10. ^ Dehaene S (1989). "The psychophysics of numerical comparison: a reexamination of apparently incompatible data". Perception & Psychophysics. 45 (6): 557–566. doi:10.3758/bf03208063. PMID 2740196.
  11. ^ Dehaene S.; Dupoux E.; Mehler J. (1990). "Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 16 (3): 626–641. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.16.3.626. PMID 2144576.
  12. ^ Dehaene S.; Bossini S.; Giraux P. (1993). "The mental representation of parity and numerical magnitude". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 122 (3): 371–396. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371.
  13. ^ Dehaene S.; Changeux J.P. (1993). "Development of elementary numerical abilities: A neuronal model". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 5 (4): 390–407. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.408.389. doi:10.1162/jocn.1993.5.4.390. PMID 23964915. S2CID 16000458.
  14. ^ Nieder A (2005). "Counting on neurons: The neurobiology of numerical competence". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 6 (3): 177–190. doi:10.1038/nrn1626. PMID 15711599. S2CID 14578049.
  15. ^ Dehaene S.; Cohen L. (1991). "Two mental calculation systems". Neuropsychologia. 29 (11): 1045–74. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(91)90076-k. PMID 1723179. S2CID 8484983.
  16. ^ Dehaene S (1996). "The organization of brain activations in number comparison: Event-related potentials and the additive-factors method". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 8 (1): 47–68. doi:10.1162/jocn.1996.8.1.47. PMID 23972235. S2CID 8546301.
  17. ^ Kiefer M.; Dehaene S. (1997). "The time course of parietal activation in single-digit multiplication: Evidence from event-related potentials". Mathematical Cognition. 3: 1–30. doi:10.1080/135467997387461.
  18. ^ Dehaene S.; Spelke L.; Pinel P.; Stanescu R.; Tsivkin S. (1999). "Sources of mathematical thinking : behavioral and brain-imaging evidence". Science. 284 (5416): 970–974. Bibcode:1999Sci...284..970D. doi:10.1126/science.284.5416.970. PMID 10320379.
  19. ^ Pinel P.; Le Clec'h G.; van de Moortele P.F.; Naccache L.; Le Bihan D.; Dehaene S. (1999). "Event-related fMRI analysis of the cerebral circuit for number comparison". NeuroReport. 10 (7): 1473–79. doi:10.1097/00001756-199905140-00015. PMID 10380965.
  20. ^ Chochon F.; Cohen L.; van de Moortele P.F.; Dehaene S. (1999). "Differential contributions of the left and right inferior parietal lobules to number processing". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 11 (6): 617–630. doi:10.1162/089892999563689. PMID 10601743. S2CID 7960805.
  21. ^ Pica, P; Lemer, C; Izard, V; Dehaene, S (2004). "Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group" (PDF). Science. 306 (5695): 499–503. Bibcode:2004Sci...306..499P. doi:10.1126/science.1102085. PMID 15486303. S2CID 10653745.
  22. ^ Dehaene S.; Naccache L. (2001). "Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework". Cognition. 79 (1–2): 1–37. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00123-2. PMID 11164022. S2CID 1762431.
  23. ^ van Vugt, B; Dagnino, B; Vartak, D; Safaai, H; Panzeri, S; Dehaene, S; Roelfsema, PR (22 March 2018). "The threshold for conscious report: Signal loss and response bias in visual and frontal cortex". Science. 360 (6388): 537–542. Bibcode:2018Sci...360..537V. doi:10.1126/science.aar7186. hdl:1871.1/50416534-5ba0-4674-8acf-8344d26f54a4. PMID 29567809.
  24. ^ Dehaene S.; Naccache L.; Cohen L.; LeBihan D.; Mangin J.F.; Poline J.-B.; Rivière D. (2001). "Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming". Nature Neuroscience. 4 (7): 752–758. doi:10.1038/89551. PMID 11426233. S2CID 16817321.
  25. ^ Sergent C.; Baillet S.; Dehaene S. (2005). "Timing of the brain events underlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink". Nature Neuroscience. 8 (10): 1285–86. doi:10.1038/nn1549. PMID 16158062. S2CID 7190211.
  26. ^ Meyen, S., Zerweck, I. A., Amado, C., von Luxburg, U., & Franz, V. H. (2021, July 15). Advancing Research on Unconscious Priming: When Can Scientists Claim an Indirect Task Advantage?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0001065
  27. ^ Cohen L, Lehéricy S, Chochon F, Lemer C, Rivaud S, Dehaene S (2002). "Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area". Brain. 125 (Pt 5): 1054–1069. doi:10.1093/brain/awf094. PMID 11960895.
  28. ^ Dehaene S, Le Clec'H G, Poline JB, Le Bihan D, Cohen L (2002). "The visual word form area: a prelexical representation of visual words in the fusiform gyrus". NeuroReport. 13 (3): 321–325. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.10.7084. doi:10.1097/00001756-200203040-00015. PMID 11930131. S2CID 17598792.
  29. ^ McCandliss BD, Cohen L, Dehaene S (2003). "The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7 (7): 293–299. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00134-7. PMID 12860187. S2CID 8534353.
  30. ^ Gaillard R, Naccache L, Pinel P, Clémenceau S, Volle E, Hasboun D, Dupont S, Baulac M, Dehaene S, Adam C, Cohen L (2006). "Direct intracranial, FMRI, and lesion evidence for the causal role of left inferotemporal cortex in reading". Neuron. 50 (2): 191–204. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.031. PMID 16630832.
  31. ^ Dehaene S, Cohen L, Sigman M, Vinckier F (2005). "The neural code for written words: a proposal". Trends Cogn Sci. 9 (7): 335–341. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.004. PMID 15951224. S2CID 17737103.
  32. ^ Vinckier F, Dehaene S, Jobert A, Dubus JP, Sigman M, Cohen L (2007). "Hierarchical coding of letter strings in the ventral stream: dissecting the inner organization of the visual word-form system". Neuron. 55 (1): 143–156. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.031. hdl:11336/67508. PMID 17610823. S2CID 2361742.
  33. ^ Dehaene S, Cohen L (2007). "Cultural recycling of cortical maps". Neuron. 56 (2): 384–398. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.004. PMID 17964253. S2CID 11364814.
  34. ^ Dehaene S, Pegado F, Braga LW, Ventura P, Nunes Filho G, Jobert A, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Kolinsky R, Morais J, Cohen L (2010). "How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language" (PDF). Science. 330 (6009): 1359–1364. Bibcode:2010Sci...330.1359D. doi:10.1126/science.1194140. PMID 21071632. S2CID 1359577.
  35. ^ Dehaene, S.; Cohen, L. (2011). "The unique role of the visual word form area in reading". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 15 (6): 254–62. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.003. PMID 21592844. S2CID 14043432.
  36. ^ "Reading in the Brain by Stanislas Dehaene". pagesperso-orange.fr. Retrieved 6 September 2010.
[edit]