P-rep
This article needs additional citations for verification. (October 2011) |
In statistical hypothesis testing, p-rep or prep has been proposed as a statistical alternative to the classic p-value.[1] Whereas a p-value is the probability of obtaining a result under the null hypothesis, p-rep purports to compute the probability of replicating an effect. The derivation of p-rep contained significant mathematical errors.
For a while, the Association for Psychological Science recommended that articles submitted to Psychological Science and their other journals report p-rep rather than the classic p-value,[2] but this is no longer the case.[3]
Calculation
[edit]This section is missing information about basic definition from Cohen's d “effect size” and why the approximation works.(July 2021) |
Approximation from p
[edit]The value of the p-rep (prep) can be approximated based on the p-value (p) as follows:
The above applies for one-tailed distributions.
Criticism
[edit]The fact that the p-rep has a one-to-one correspondence with the p-value makes it clear that this new measure brings no additional information beyond that conveyed by the significance of the result. Killeen acknowledges this lack of information, but suggests that p-rep better captures the way naive experimenters conceptualize p-values and statistical hypothesis testing.
Among the criticisms of p-rep is the fact that while it attempts to estimate replicability, it ignores results from other studies which can accurately guide this estimate.[4] For example, an experiment on some unlikely paranormal phenomenon may yield a p-rep of 0.75. Most people would still not conclude the probability of a replication was 75%. Rather, they would conclude it is much closer to 0: Extraordinary claims require extraordinary evidence, and p-rep ignores this. Because of this, p-rep may in fact be harder to interpret than a classical p-value. The fact that p-rep requires assumptions about prior probabilities for it to be valid makes its interpretation complex. Killeen argues that new results should be evaluated in their own right, without the "burden of history", with flat priors: that is what p-rep yields. A more pragmatic estimate of replicability would include prior knowledge, via, for instance, meta-analysis.
Critics have also underscored mathematical errors in the original Killeen paper. For example, the formula relating the effect sizes from two replications of a given experiment erroneously uses one of these random variables as a parameter of the probability distribution of the other while he previously hypothesized these two variables to be independent,[5] criticisms addressed in Killeen's rejoinder.[6]
A further criticism of the p-rep statistic involves the logic of experimentation. The scientific value of replicable data lies in the adequate accounting for previously unmeasured factors (e.g., unmeasured participant variables, experimenter's bias, etc.), The idea that a single study can capture a logical likelihood of such unmeasured factors affecting the outcome, and thus the likelihood of replicability, is a logical fallacy.[citation needed]
References
[edit]- ^ Killeen PR (2005). "An alternative to null-hypothesis significance tests". Psychological Science. 16 (5): 345–53. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01538.x. PMC 1473027. PMID 15869691.
- ^ archived version of "Psychological Science Journal, Author Guidelines"
- ^ Psychological Science Journal, Author Guidelines.
- ^ Macdonald, R. R. (2005) "Why Replication Probabilities Depend on Prior Probability Distributions" Psychological Science, 2005, 16, 1006–1008 [https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2005-15678-016
- ^ "p-rep" at Pro Bono Statistics
- ^ Killeen, P. R. (2005)" Replicability, Confidence, and Priors", Psychological Science, 2005, 16, 1009–1012 [1]
External links
[edit]- Schimmack, Ulrich (19 September 2017). "P-REP (2005-2009): Reexamining the experiment to replace p-values with the probability of replicating an effect". Replicability-Index.