Laughter in animals
Laughter in animals other than humans describes animal behavior which resembles human laughter.
Several non-human species demonstrate vocalizations that sound similar to human laughter. A significant proportion of these species are mammals, which suggests that the neurological functions occurred early in the process of mammalian evolution.[1] Laughter as communication is found in over 60 species.[2]
Apes
[edit]Chimpanzees, gorillas and orangutans show laughter-like vocalizations in response to physical contact such as wrestling, play chasing or tickling. Some orangutans also react with laughter at magic tricks performed by humans.[3][4] This behavior is documented in both wild and captive chimpanzees. Chimpanzee laughter is not readily recognizable to humans as such, because it is generated by alternating inhalations and exhalations that sound more like breathing and panting.[5] It sounds similar to screeching. The differences between chimpanzee and human laughter may be the result of adaptations that have evolved to enable human speech. One study analyzed sounds made by human babies and bonobos when tickled. It found that although the bonobo's laugh was a higher frequency, the laugh followed the same sonographic pattern as human babies and included similar facial expressions. Humans and chimpanzees share similar ticklish areas of the body such as the armpits and belly.[6]
Research has noted the similarity in forms of laughter among humans and other apes (chimpanzees, gorillas and orangutans) when tickled, suggesting that laughter derived from a common origin among primate species, and therefore evolved prior to the origin of humans.[7][8]
Rats
[edit]Rats emit long, 50-kHz ultrasonic calls that are induced during rough and tumble play, and when tickled by humans. The vocalization is described as distinct "chirping". Like humans, rats have "tickle skin", areas of the body that generate greater laughter responses than others. Rats that laugh the most also play the most and prefer to spend more time with other laughing rats. It has been reported that there is no decline in the tendency to laugh and respond to tickle skin as rats age, however, it has also been reported that in females, brain maturation after puberty appears to redefine tickling as aversive, leading to avoidance rather than appetitive responses.[9] Further studies show that rats chirp when wrestling one another, before receiving morphine, or when mating. The sound has been interpreted as an expectation of something rewarding.[10] High frequency ultrasonic vocalizations are important in rat communication and function to elicit approach behavior in the recipient.[11]
The initial goal of research by Jaak Panksepp and Jeff Burgdorf was to track the biological origins of how the brain processes emotions and social behavior. They compared rat vocalizations during social interactions to the joy and laughter commonly experienced by children in social play. They concluded that the 50-kHz rat vocalizations might reflect positive affective states (feelings or emotions), analogous to those experienced by children laughing during social play.[12]
More recent studies have investigated the emotional states of rats after being tickled. An animal's optimism or pessimism can be assessed by cognitive bias studies. After being tickled, rats are more optimistic, indicating the interaction invokes a positive affective state.[13] Furthermore, rats self-administer playback of the 50-kHz trill calls and avoid playback of 22-kHz calls.[14]
When rats are given naloxone (an opioid antagonist), tickling no longer evokes the 50-kHz vocalisation[citation needed] which indicates that the rewarding properties of tickling are modulated by endogenous opioids.
Dogs
[edit]Dogs sometimes pant in a manner that sounds like a human laugh. By analyzing the pant using a sonograph, this pant varies with bursts of frequencies. When this vocalization is played to dogs in a shelter setting, it can initiate play, promote pro-social behavior, and decrease stress levels. One study compared the behaviour of 120 dogs with and without exposure to a recorded "dog-laugh". Playback reduced stress-related behaviors, increased tail wagging, the display of a "play-face" when playing was initiated, and pro-social behavior such as approaching and lip licking.[15]
Dolphins
[edit]In 2004, researchers who were studying dolphins in Sweden noticed a particular set of sounds that they had not heard before.[16] These sounds consisted of a short burst of pulses, followed by a whistle. After further observations the researchers discovered that these signals were only being made by dolphins during play-fighting, and never during aggressive confrontations. Their conclusion was that these sounds were being made by the dolphins to indicate that the situation was pleasant and/or non-threatening, and to help prevent it escalating into something like a real fight. This, according to psychologists, is the reason why laughter exists in the first place, suggesting these noises were the dolphin equivalent of a human laugh.[17]
Donkeys
[edit]Donkeys are known to produce a unique vocalization called "braying," often described as a series of loud, distinct sounds. While this braying is typically a form of communication related to social interactions or environmental responses, recent studies suggest donkeys might also exhibit specific vocalizations in playful contexts that resemble other animals' laughter-like behaviors. Although donkeys do not "laugh" in the traditional sense, their playful vocalizations and interactions, such as nuzzling or mock-biting with other donkeys, may indicate positive emotional states similar to those observed in other animals that demonstrate laughter-like behaviors. Further research is needed to clarify if this braying serves any function similar to laughter in other species. Recent anecdotal evidence, such as videos shared online, shows donkeys making vocalizations that resemble human laughter.
References
[edit]- ^ Panksepp, J. (2000). "The riddle of laughter neural and psychoevolutionary underpinnings of joy". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 9 (6): 183–186. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00090. S2CID 145577390.
- ^ Johnson, Doug (17 May 2021). "From apes to birds, there are 65 animal species that "laugh"". Ars Technica. Retrieved 20 May 2021.
- ^ "Watch an Orangutan "Laugh" Hysterically at a Zoo Visitor's Magic Trick". Time. 10 December 2015. Retrieved 8 December 2023.
- ^ "Orangutan laughs hysterically at zoo magic trick - and so will you - CBS News". www.cbsnews.com. 16 December 2015. Retrieved 8 December 2023.
- ^ "Apes Laugh, Tickle Study Finds". National Geographic. Archived from the original on June 7, 2009. Retrieved 2018-05-31.
- ^ "Where did laughter come from?". ABC Science. October 7, 2003.
- ^ "Tickled apes yield laughter clue", BBC, June 4, 2009
- ^ Davila Ross, Marina; j Owren, Michael; Zimmermann, Elke (2009). "Reconstructing the Evolution of Laughter in Great Apes and Humans". Current Biology. 19 (13): 1106–1111. Bibcode:2009CBio...19.1106D. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.05.028. PMID 19500987. S2CID 17892549.
- ^ Paredes-Ramos, P.; Miquel, M.; Manzo, J.; Pfaus, J.G.; López-Meraz, M.L.; Coria-Avila, G.A. (2012). "Tickling in juvenile but not adult female rats conditions sexual partner preference". Physiology & Behavior. 107 (1): 17–25. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.05.017. PMID 22640704. S2CID 161288.
- ^ Science News 2001 - requires signup
- ^ Wöhr, M.; Schwarting, R.K. (2007). "Ultrasonic communication in rats: Can playback of 50-kHz calls induce approach behavior?". PLOS ONE. 2 (12): e1365. Bibcode:2007PLoSO...2.1365W. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001365. PMC 2137933. PMID 18159248.
- ^ Panksepp, J.; Burgdorf, J. (2003). ""Laughing" rats and the evolutionary antecedents of human joy?" (PDF). Physiology & Behavior. 79 (3): 533–547. doi:10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00159-8. PMID 12954448. S2CID 14063615. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-11-25. Retrieved 2014-08-06.
- ^ Rygula, R.; Pluta, H.; Popik, P. (2012). "Laughing rats are optimistic". PLOS ONE. 7 (12): e51959. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...751959R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0051959. PMC 3530570. PMID 23300582.
- ^ Burgdorf, J.; Kroes, R.A.; Moskal, J.R.; Pfaus, J.G.; Brudzynski, S.M.; Panksepp, J. (2008). "Ultrasonic vocalizations of rats (Rattus norvegicus) during mating, play, and aggression: Behavioral concomitants, relationship to reward, and self-administration of playback". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 122 (4): 357–367. doi:10.1037/a0012889. PMID 19014259.
- ^ Simonet, P.; Versteeg, D. & Storie, D. (2005). "Dog-laughter: Recorded playback reduces stress related behavior in shelter dogs" (PDF). Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Environmental Enrichment.
- ^ "An Acoustic Play-Fight Signal in Bottlenose Dolphins in Human Care" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on March 7, 2022.
- ^ "Tickling rats and giggling dolphins: Do animals have a sense of humour?". TheGuardian.com. 17 November 2015.
External links
[edit]- Simonet 2005 (includes a sample of dog laughter)
- Goodall 1968 & Parr 2005 (a sample of chimpanzee laughter)