Draft:Trust Game
Submission declined on 27 July 2024 by Asilvering (talk). Thank you for your submission, but the subject of this article already exists in Wikipedia. You can find it and improve it at Dictator game#Variants instead.
Where to get help
How to improve a draft
You can also browse Wikipedia:Featured articles and Wikipedia:Good articles to find examples of Wikipedia's best writing on topics similar to your proposed article. Improving your odds of a speedy review To improve your odds of a faster review, tag your draft with relevant WikiProject tags using the button below. This will let reviewers know a new draft has been submitted in their area of interest. For instance, if you wrote about a female astronomer, you would want to add the Biography, Astronomy, and Women scientists tags. Editor resources
|
- Comment: Few reviewers will have access to these sources, so if you can add accessible sources that will improve the likelihood of a review being able to establish notability. Greenman (talk) 12:36, 24 June 2024 (UTC)
The trust game is one of the foundational games in experimental economics and behavioral economics.[1]
In the basic two-player game, there are two players, each of whom is endowed with an equal initial endowment.[2]
One of the two players is a first mover-- sometimes referred to as the Trustor-- who chooses between Invest and Not Invest. Not investing guarantees the first mover his initial endowment as a sure outcome. Investing transfer the entire invested amount by the first mover to the second mover, multiplied by 3. That invested amount is subtracted from the first mover's endowment.[3]
If the first mover invests, the second mover-- referred to as a Trustee-- receives the entire investment of the fist mover, multiplied by 3. He can choose how much to transfer back to the first mover.[4]
If the second mover transfer any amount over 1/3 of the investment to the first mover, then the first mover is as well off as he would have been had he not invested. However, the second mover has a strategic dominance in the sub-game of transferring back nothing at all.[5][6]
By backward induction, the first mover can expect to receive nothing, and therefore he is better off not investing.[7]
The trust game is sometimes erroneously referred to as a variant of the dictator game.[8]
References
[edit]- ^ Burks, Stephen V.; Carpenter, Jeffrey P.; Verhoogen, Eric (June 2003). "Playing both roles in the trust game". Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization. 51 (2): 195–216. doi:10.1016/S0167-2681(02)00093-8.
- ^ Johnson, Noel D.; Mislin, Alexandra A. (October 2011). "Trust games: A meta-analysis". Journal of Economic Psychology. 32 (5): 865–889. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2011.05.007.
- ^ Wilson, Rick K.; Eckel, Catherine C. (June 2006). "Judging a Book by its Cover: Beauty and Expectations in the Trust Game". Political Research Quarterly. 59 (2): 189–202. doi:10.1177/106591290605900202. ISSN 1065-9129.
- ^ Berg, Joyce; Dickhaut, John; McCabe, Kevin (July 1995). "Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History". Games and Economic Behavior. 10 (1): 122–142. doi:10.1006/game.1995.1027.
- ^ Krueger, Frank; McCabe, Kevin; Moll, Jorge; Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus; Zahn, Roland; Strenziok, Maren; Heinecke, Armin; Grafman, Jordan (2007-12-11). "Neural correlates of trust". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104 (50): 20084–20089. Bibcode:2007PNAS..10420084K. doi:10.1073/pnas.0710103104. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 2148426. PMID 18056800.
- ^ Rigdon, Mary L.; McCabe, Kevin A.; Smith, Vernon L. (2007). "Sustaining Cooperation in Trust Games". The Economic Journal. 117 (522): 991–1007. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0297.2007.02075.x. ISSN 0013-0133. JSTOR 4625540.
- ^ Burnham, Terence; McCabe, Kevin; Smith, Vernon L (September 2000). "Friend-or-foe intentionality priming in an extensive form trust game". Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization. 43 (1): 57–73. doi:10.1016/S0167-2681(00)00108-6.
- ^ Alarcon, Gene M.; Lyons, Joseph B.; Christensen, James C.; Klosterman, Samantha L.; Bowers, Margaret A.; Ryan, Tyler J.; Jessup, Sarah A.; Wynne, Kevin T. (October 2018). "The effect of propensity to trust and perceptions of trustworthiness on trust behaviors in dyads". Behavior Research Methods. 50 (5): 1906–1920. doi:10.3758/s13428-017-0959-6. ISSN 1554-3528. PMID 28917031.